Power, Control, and Influence in Sibling Relationships across Development -  - E-Book

Power, Control, and Influence in Sibling Relationships across Development E-Book

0,0
22,99 €

-100%
Sammeln Sie Punkte in unserem Gutscheinprogramm und kaufen Sie E-Books und Hörbücher mit bis zu 100% Rabatt.
Mehr erfahren.
Beschreibung

Take a close look at sibling relationships--particularly how siblings navigate power, control, and influence and how the relationship affects the development of the individuals involved. While such relationships are both complementary and reciprocal, they transforms rather dramatically: from hierarchical in early and middle childhood, to egalitarian by early adulthood. This issue to examines: * the processes and consequences of such dynamic power shifts for our broader understanding of how these relationship dynamics change and develop throughout the life course, * how such dynamics may be similar or different cross-culturally, and * how they influence the quality of the sibling relationship, as well as the well-being of youth. To do so, the authors present research from each developmental period from early childhood through young adulthood, as well as cross-cultural research, in order to further understanding of the developmental and contextual themes that allow for drawing broader conclusions. This is the 156th volume in this Jossey-Bass series New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development. Its mission is to provide scientific and scholarly presentations on cutting edge issues and concepts in this subject area. Each volume focuses on a specific new direction or research topic and is edited by experts from that field.

Sie lesen das E-Book in den Legimi-Apps auf:

Android
iOS
von Legimi
zertifizierten E-Readern

Seitenzahl: 230

Veröffentlichungsjahr: 2017

Bewertungen
0,0
0
0
0
0
0
Mehr Informationen
Mehr Informationen
Legimi prüft nicht, ob Rezensionen von Nutzern stammen, die den betreffenden Titel tatsächlich gekauft oder gelesen/gehört haben. Wir entfernen aber gefälschte Rezensionen.



New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development

Elena L.Grigorenko EDITOR-IN-CHIEF

William Damon FOUNDING EDITOR

Power, Control, and Influence in Sibling Relationships across Development

Nicole Campione-Barr EDITOR

Number 156 • Summer 2017

Jossey-Bass

San Francisco

Power, Control, and Influence in Sibling Relationships across Development

Nicole Campione‐Barr (Ed.)

New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, no. 156

Editor‐in‐Chief: Elena L. Grigorenko

New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development (Print ISSN: 1520‐3247; Online ISSN: 1534‐8687), is published quarterly by Wiley Subscription Services, Inc., a Wiley Company, 111 River St., Hoboken, NJ 07030‐5774 USA.

Postmaster: Send all address changes to New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, John Wiley & Sons Inc., C/O The Sheridan Press, PO Box 465, Hanover, PA 17331 USA.

Copyright and Copying (in any format)

Copyright © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc., a Wiley Company. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored or transmitted in any form or by any means without the prior permission in writing from the copyright holder. Authorization to copy items for internal and personal use is granted by the copyright holder for libraries and other users registered with their local Reproduction Rights Organisation (RRO), e.g. Copyright Clearance Center (CCC), 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, USA (www.copyright.com), provided the appropriate fee is paid directly to the RRO. This consent does not extend to other kinds of copying such as copying for general distribution, for advertising or promotional purposes, for republication, for creating new collective works or for resale. Permissions for such reuse can be obtained using the RightsLink “Request Permissions” link on Wiley Online Library. Special requests should be addressed to: permissions@wiley.com

Information for subscribers

New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development is published in 4 issues per year. Institutional subscription prices for 2017 are:

Print & Online: US$562 (US), US$617 (Canada & Mexico), US$663 (Rest of World), €435 (Europe), £347 (UK). Prices are exclusive of tax. Asia‐Pacific GST, Canadian GST/HST and European VAT will be applied at the appropriate rates. For more information on current tax rates, please go to www.wileyonlinelibrary.com/tax-vat. The price includes online access to the current and all online back‐files to January 1st 2013, where available. For other pricing options, including access information and terms and conditions, please visit www.wileyonlinelibrary.com/access.

Delivery Terms and Legal Title

Where the subscription price includes print issues and delivery is to the recipient's address, delivery terms are Delivered at Place (DAP); the recipient is responsible for paying any import duty or taxes. Title to all issues transfers FOB our shipping point, freight prepaid. We will endeavour to fulfill claims for missing or damaged copies within six months of publication, within our reasonable discretion and subject to availability.

Back issues: Single issues from current and recent volumes are available at the current single issue price from cs‐journals@wiley.com.

Disclaimer

The Publisher and Editors cannot be held responsible for errors or any consequences arising from the use of information contained in this journal; the views and opinions expressed do not necessarily reflect those of the Publisher and Editors, neither does the publication of advertisements constitute any endorsement by the Publisher and Editors of the products advertised.

Publisher: New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development is published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc., 350 Main St., Malden, MA 02148‐5020.

Journal Customer Services: For ordering information, claims and any enquiry concerning your journal subscription please go to www.wileycustomerhelp.com/ask or contact your nearest office.

Americas: Email: cs‐journals@wiley.com; Tel: +1 781 388 8598 or +1 800 835 6770 (toll free in the USA & Canada).

Europe, Middle East and Africa: Email: cs‐journals@wiley.com; Tel: +44 (0) 1865 778315.

Asia Pacific: Email: cs‐journals@wiley.com; Tel: +65 6511 8000.

Japan: For Japanese speaking support, Email: cs‐japan@wiley.com.

Visit our Online Customer Help available in 7 languages at www.wileycustomerhelp.com/ask

Production Editor: Shreya Srivastava (email: shsrivasta@wiley.com).

Wiley's Corporate Citizenship initiative seeks to address the environmental, social, economic, and ethical challenges faced in our business and which are important to our diverse stakeholder groups. Since launching the initiative, we have focused on sharing our content with those in need, enhancing community philanthropy, reducing our carbon impact, creating global guidelines and best practices for paper use, establishing a vendor code of ethics, and engaging our colleagues and other stakeholders in our efforts. Follow our progress at www.wiley.com/go/citizenship

View this journal online at wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/cad

Wiley is a founding member of the UN‐backed HINARI, AGORA, and OARE initiatives. They are now collectively known as Research4Life, making online scientific content available free or at nominal cost to researchers in developing countries. Please visit Wiley's Content Access ‐ Corporate Citizenship site: http://www.wiley.com/WileyCDA/Section/id-390082.html

Address for Editorial Correspondence: Editor‐in‐chief, Elena L. Grigorenko, New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, Email: [email protected]

Abstracting and Indexing Services

The Journal is indexed by Academic Search Alumni Edition (EBSCO Publishing); ASSIA: Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ProQuest); Environmental Sciences & Pollution Management (ProQuest); ERA: Educational Research Abstracts Online (T&F); ERIC: Educational Resources Information Center (CSC); Health & Safety Science Abstracts (ProQuest); Linguistics & Language Behavior Abstracts (ProQuest); MEDLINE/PubMed (NLM); Pollution Abstracts (ProQuest); Professional Development Collection (EBSCO Publishing); PsycINFO/Psychological Abstracts (APA); Safety Science & Risk Abstracts (ProQuest); Social Services Abstracts (ProQuest); SocINDEX (EBSCO Publishing); Sociological Abstracts (ProQuest).

Cover design: Wiley

Cover Images: © Lava 4 images | Shutterstock

For submission instructions, subscription and all other information visit:

wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/cad

Elena L. Grigorenko, Editor-in-Chief

University of Houston, TX

Editorial Board

Baptiste Barbot, PhD Pace University, USA

David D. Preiss, PhD Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Chile

Linda Jarvin, PhD Paris College of Art, France

Peggy McCardle, PhD, MPH Peggy McCardle Consulting, LLC, USA

Fumiko Hoeft, MD, PhD University of California San Francisco, USA

Jens F. Beckmann, Dr. rer. nat. Durham University, UK

CONTENTS

1: The Changing Nature of Power, Control, and Influence in Sibling Relationships

How Is Relational Power in Sibling Relationships Different From Other Relationships?

Sources of Power in Sibling Relationships and Their Impacts

Development and Context of Power, Control, and Influence in Sibling Relationships

Conclusions

References

2: Siblings’ Power and Influence in Polyadic Family Conflict During Early Childhood

Sibling Relationships in Early Childhood

Siblings as Active Agents in Polyadic Conflict

Influential and Operational Elements of Polyadic Family Conflict

Present Study

Method

Results

Discussion

Conclusion

References

3: Family Perspectives on Siblings’ Conflict Goals in Middle Childhood: Links to Hierarchical and Affective Features of Sibling Relationships

The Motivational Underpinnings of Sibling Disputes

Power, Control, and Asymmetry Between Siblings in Middle Childhood

Family Members’ Perspectives on Conflicts Between Siblings

The Current Study

Method

Results

Discussion

Conclusions

Author Note

Notes

References

4: Relative Power in Sibling Relationships Across Adolescence

Developmental Trends in Adolescent Sibling Relative Power

The Present Study

Method

Results

Discussion

Limitations, Future Directions, and Conclusions

References

5: Sibling Influences on Risky Behaviors from Adolescence to Young Adulthood: Vertical Socialization or Bidirectional Effects?

Sibling Socialization Processes

Sibling Socialization Effects Across Adolescence and Young Adulthood

Current Study

Method

Results

Discussion

References

6: Cross-Cultural Differences in Sibling Power Balance and Its Concomitants Across Three Age Periods

Sibling Power Balance and Psychosocial Functioning

Cross-Cultural Differences in Sibling Power Balance

The Role of Age and Gender in Sibling Power Balance

The Present Study

Study 1: Early Childhood

Study 2: Late Childhood

Study 3: Adolescence

Discussion

Conclusions and Implications

Author Note

References

7: Commentary 1: The Challenging Task of Addressing Power, Control, and Influence in Sibling Relationships After the First Two Decades of Life

Macro Influences on Power

Adult Sibling Interactions

Conclusion

References

8: Commentary 2: Sibling Power Dynamics: The Role of Family and Sociocultural Context

References

Order Form

Index

End User License Agreement

List of Tables

Chapter 2

Table 2.1

Table 2.2

Table 2.3

Table 2.4

Chapter 3

Table 3.1

Chapter 4

Table 4.1

Table 4.2

Chapter 5

Table 5.1

Table 5.2

Table 5.3

Chapter 6

Table 6.1

Table 6.2

Table 6.3

List of Illustrations

Chapter 4

Figure 4.1

Longitudinal trajectory of sibling relative power by birth order. **

p

< 0.01

Figure 4.2

Longitudinal trajectory of sibling relative power by positivity level. **

p

< 0.01

Chapter 5

Figure 5.1

The association between secondborns’ deviant behavior and firstborns’ deviant behavior as moderated by age. *

p

< 0.05. **

p

< 0.01. ***

p

< 0.001

Figure 5.2

The association between firstborns’ deviant behavior and seconborns’ deviant behavior as moderated by age. *

p

< 0.05. **

p

< 0.01. ***

p

< 0.001

Guide

Cover

Table of Contents

1

Pages

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

33

34

35

36

37

38

39

40

42

43

44

45

46

47

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

58

59

60

61

62

63

64

65

66

67

68

69

70

71

72

73

75

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

83

84

85

87

88

89

90

91

92

93

94

95

96

97

98

99

100

101

102

103

104

105

106

107

108

109

110

111

112

113

114

115

Campione‐Barr, N. (2017). The changing nature of power, control, and influence in sibling relationships. In N. Campione‐Barr (Ed.), Power, control, and influence in sibling relationships across development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 156, 7–14.

1

The Changing Nature of Power, Control, and Influence in Sibling Relationships

Nicole Campione-Barr

Abstract

The sibling relationship is unique in that it transforms across development from hierarchical in early childhood, to egalitarian by adulthood. The present article reviews the previous theorizing and research literature regarding how and why power, control, and therefore sibling influence, change over the course of the first couple of decades, and introduces the goals and advancements made by the new research presented in this issue. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

The sibling relationship is unique in comparison to other close relationships in several ways, but it is the developmentally changing nature of power dynamics that are likely the most salient. In other familial relationships, younger generations are generally expected to defer to older generations, regardless of age. Friendships and romantic relationships, in contrast, are generally equal in power, control, and decision making across time. Therefore, sibling relationships are unique because they change from hierarchical in early and middle childhood, to more egalitarian by adulthood. Additionally, such evolving power dynamics have an impact on how control and influence are exerted within the dyad. Therefore, the objective of this issue is to examine the processes and consequences of sibling relational power, to understand better how sibling relationship dynamics change and develop throughout the life course, how such dynamics may be similar or different cross culturally, and how they influence both the quality of the sibling relationship, as well as the well-being of youth.

This issue also both builds upon, and differs from, the 2009 New Directions issue edited by Kramer and Conger, and the last time New Directions focused on the role of siblings in child and adolescent development. That issue emphasized the ways siblings serve as socializing agents for both prosocial and antisocial behavior in youth. Although the control and influence siblings hold over one another are largely due to the power dynamics present within each dyad, and may also serve as a mechanism by which siblings serve as socializing agents, power differentials between siblings are also important for the quality of the relationship, youth social development, and adjustment. Therefore, further investigation into this mechanism, which changes dramatically over two decades, is important for improving our understanding of this relatively understudied social relationship. In addition, the present issue engages several early career researchers, in concert with well-known experts in the field, as a reflection of the growth of the field of sibling relationship scholarship. Thus, the present article reviews the role of power in sibling relationships specifically, and the following ones expand on the examination of the development of relational power, control, and influence from early childhood through young adulthood.

How Is Relational Power in Sibling Relationships Different From Other Relationships?

Laursen and Bukowski (1997) contend that three discrete dimensions can aid in distinguishing different close relationships from one another: permanence (i.e., voluntary vs. obligatory), power (i.e., hierarchical vs. egalitarian), and gender (i.e., same-sex vs. mixed-sex). Sibling relationships are obligatory in their permanence, and can be either same-sex or mixed-sex in their makeup, but it is the role of power within the sibling relationship that is the primary focus of the present issue. Although the relational dimension of power within close relationships can typically be described in terms of whether or not a relationship is “hierarchical or egalitarian, vertical or horizontal, authoritative or mutual” (Laursen & Bukowski, 1997, p. 753), sibling relationships are unique in that they can be described by all of these features.

Sibling dyads are hierarchical in that, with the exception of twins, the age difference and order of appearance in the family dictate a formal rank ordering. Therefore, older siblings are considered to be physically, socially, and cognitively advantaged over their younger siblings. However, sibling relationships also become more egalitarian with age and development. As younger siblings become more equally matched in these capacities with their older siblings, the expectation that interactions will become more equal increases (Dunn, 2002; Fiske, 1992; Laursen & Bukowski, 1997). Sibling relationships are also vertical in that they are complimentary; more capable older siblings are often responsible for the caretaking of less capable younger siblings. Even from an early age, however, these relationships are also horizontal in that they are reciprocal. This particularly occurs through sibling play interactions, where turn-taking and equal participation is expected (Dunn, 2002; Laursen & Bukowski, 1997). Finally, authoritative relationship structures assume that the direction of influence between members is typically one-sided (Laursen & Bukowski, 1997). In the case of sibling relationships, much of the research on sibling socialization and influence has found that older siblings are typically more influential on younger siblings than the reverse. However, mutual interactions, in which collaboration and bidirectional influence occur, are also frequent between siblings (Kramer & Conger, 2009; Whiteman, Bernard, & Jensen, 2011; Whiteman, Jensen & McHale, this issue).

Power dynamics within sibling relationships can be complicated, and much more so than those with parents (which are typically hierarchical, vertical, and authoritative) or with peers (which are typically egalitarian, horizontal, and mutual). Therefore, the unique blend of complimentary and reciprocal interactions between siblings likely influence the broader dynamic and quality of the relationship more so than with other close relationships where the power dynamics are more consistent and stable. Such volatility in power dynamics, and changes in power processes over the course of development, however, have an impact on how siblings handle conflicts, attempt to control one another, and influence each other's behaviors.

Sources of Power in Sibling Relationships and Their Impacts

Based on the French and Raven (1959) typology of social power, Perlman, Siddiqui, Ram, and Ross (2000) identified five sources of power during youths’ conflictive interactions. Given the relatively high level of conflict that occurs among siblings throughout childhood and adolescence (Kim, McHale, Osgood, & Crouter, 2006), sibling posturing for power and control likely occurs most often during conflict (and is further examined in the Della Porta & Howe; Recchia & Witwit, this issue). Therefore, it is important to consider the sources of power that are evident in the context of conflict and negotiation, particularly for siblings.

The ability to coerce or reward social partners likely comes with maturity; so older siblings are more effective in this regard than younger siblings. Older siblings are more likely to use their power to win conflicts, most frequently by use of aggression or rewards, particularly when siblings are younger, and when the age difference between siblings is larger. Dyads with smaller age and power discrepancies, however, lead to more frequent conflict (Vandell & Bailey, 1992). Because of older siblings’ greater physical prowess and intellect over their younger siblings, particularly at earlier ages and when greater age differences are evident, older siblings are more likely to win arguments or control interactions by expert power (e.g., when one dyad member has superior ability or knowledge over the other; Perlman et al., 2000).

Referent power occurs when siblings are invested in the welfare of the other and is less influenced by birth order. Siblings who are warmer and more supportive towards one another are more likely to try to resolve discrepancies. When older siblings care about the internal states of their younger siblings, they are more likely to grant them leverage and power in interactions, which leads to more balanced power between siblings (e.g., Ram & Ross, 2008). Alternatively, information power, or persuasive power, is engaged in with the exchange of information or logical reasoning. Older siblings are more likely to have greater knowledge, or be better at forming a logical argument than younger siblings, because of their more advanced cognitive abilities. However, in an effort to even the playing field, younger siblings are more likely to use teasing to irritate older siblings, or elicit the assistance of parents (e.g., tattling) to gain power in disputes (Perlman et al., 2000).

Finally, legitimate power occurs when opposing parties have rights and obligations to one another based on social norms and moral rules. Perlman and colleagues (2000) suggest this is the most likely form of power to prevail in sibling relationships because of the hierarchical nature of the family and cultural prescriptions that we base power on age. Thus, power may be prescribed by parents (e.g., babysitting) or general social norms that younger members of society should take direction from older members. Therefore, older siblings are generally advantaged in this situation; however, research suggests that ownership trumps birth order in disputes over personal property (Ross, 1996).

Development and Context of Power, Control, and Influence in Sibling Relationships

Early Childhood

The power imbalance between siblings is likely most pronounced during early childhood. This is because of how rapidly children develop physically and cognitively during this period. Thus, even a couple-of-year age gap between siblings at this stage can lead to large discrepancies between older and younger siblings’ size, strength, maturity, communication skills, and knowledge. Parents are also more likely to intervene in sibling disputes at earlier ages, often to support the younger, less-powerful child, and to aid siblings in finding more constructive ways to handle disputes (Perlman & Ross, 1997; Siddiqui & Ross, 1999). In this issue, Della Porta and Howe examined young siblings’ (4- and 6-year-olds) power strategies and power effectiveness during multifamily member observed conflicts. Their findings suggest that power strategies are differentially effective in siblings’ attempts to “win” conflicts based on the birth order of the children.

Middle Childhood

By middle childhood, siblings spend more time together than with any other relationship partner (Tucker, McHale, & Crouter, 2008). Additionally, by middle childhood, although a power imbalance still exists between older and younger siblings, younger siblings’ growing social competence makes them more likely to push back against older siblings’ power assertions. Doing so, however, also leads to increases in sibling conflict (until it peaks in early adolescence; Buhrmester & Furman, 1990; Kim et al., 2006). Recchia and Witwit (this issue) investigated how older and younger siblings’ (ranging in age from 4 to 10 years) conflict goal perceptions differed based on the hierarchical nature of the sibling relationship during middle childhood, as well as how these perceptions impacted the quality of the sibling relationship.

Adolescence and Early Adulthood

Early cross-sectional work by Buhrmester and Furman (1990; Furman & Buhrmester, 1992) suggested that the relative power imbalance between older and younger siblings significantly decreases over the course of adolescence. A more recent longitudinal study has found that although older adolescent siblings do continue to enjoy more power in the relationship than their younger siblings, this gap is significantly reduced over time (Tucker, Updegraff, & Baril, 2010). More egalitarian relationships are thought to be realized by emerging or young adulthood in order for siblings to function as sources of support for each other and their aging parents (Aquilino, 2006). Therefore, given the importance of developing a more egalitarian relationship by adulthood, Lindell and Campione-Barr (this issue) examined the moderating role of sibling relationship qualities (positive and negative) in the changing dynamics of sibling relative power over the course of adolescence. Additionally, although movement towards a more egalitarian relationship in early adulthood is considered both developmentally appropriate and functional for the family system, it is also possible that the direction of sibling influence, which most typically flows from older siblings to younger siblings earlier in development, may become more bidirectional. As one possible example, Whiteman, Jensen, and McHale (this issue) investigated the potential bidirectional associations of older and younger siblings’ influence on one another's risky behaviors.

Cultural Context

The vast majority of studies on sibling relational processes have been examined in Western cultures (and typically, White, middle-class, U.S. families). The minority of studies that examine other cultures suggest that there may be some differences in sibling relational processes and roles cross culturally (e.g., Updegraff, McHale, Killoren, & Rodriguez, 2011), and some similarities (e.g., Buist & Vermande, 2014; Buist et al., 2016). However, cross-cultural examinations of sibling power dynamics specifically have been sparse. Buist and colleagues (this issue) investigated the associations between sibling power dynamics and psychosocial functioning across three different developmental periods (early childhood, middle childhood, and adolescence) and from Dutch, Turkish, Indian, and Moroccan samples.

Conclusions

The unique nature of the sibling relationship often gives rise to both positive (e.g., warmth, affection) and negative (e.g., conflict, antagonism) relational qualities. It is important, then, to understand how such an ambivalent relationship can be so influential in shaping the individuals we become (Kramer & Conger, 2009). The present issue suggests that a primary mechanism by which these processes may occur is through the changing nature of sibling relational power, control, and influence throughout development. By gaining a better understanding of the ways in which siblings exert (or claim) power over one another and their interactions, we may also be able to find better targets for relationship intervention to improve sibling and family dynamics, as well as individual youth well-being.

References

Aquilino, W. S. (2006). Family relationships and support systems in emerging adulthood. In J. Jensen & J. L. Tanner (Eds.),

Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century

(pp. 193–217). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/11381-008

Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1990). Perceptions of sibling relationships during middle childhood and adolescence.

Child Development

,

61

, 1387–1398.

https://doi.org/10.2307/1130750

Buist, K. L., & Vermande, M. (2014). Sibling relationship patterns and their associations with child competence and problem behavior.

Journal of Family Psychology

,

28

, 529.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036990

Buist, K. L., Verhoeven, M., Hoksbergen, R., ter Laak, J., Watve, S., & Paranjpe, A. (2016). Associations of perceived sibling and parent–child relationship quality with internalizing and externalizing problems comparing Indian and Dutch early adolescents.

The Journal of Early Adolescence

.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431616653473

Dunn, J. (2002).

Sibling relationships

. Hoboken, NJ: Blackwell Publishing.

Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations.

Psychological Review

,

99

, 689.

https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.99.4.689

French, J. R. P., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases of social power. In D. P. Cartwright (Ed.),

Studies in social power

(pp. 150–167). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan.

Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in perceptions of networks of personal relationships.

Child Development

,

63

, 103–115.

https://doi.org/10.2307/1130905

Kim, J. Y., McHale, S. M., Osgood, D. W., & Crouter, A. C. (2006). Longitudinal course and family correlates of sibling relationships from childhood through adolescence.

Child Development

,

77

, 1746–1761.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00971.x

Kramer, L., & Conger, K. J. (2009). What we learn from our sisters and brothers: For better or for worse.

New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development

,

126

, 1–12.

https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.253

Laursen, B., & Bukowski, W. M. (1997). A developmental guide to the organisation of close relationships.

International Journal of Behavioral Development

,

21

, 747–770.

https://doi.org/10.1080/016502597384659

Perlman, M., & Ross, H. S. (1997). Who's the boss? Parents’ failed attempts to influence the outcomes of conflicts between their children.

Journal of Social and Personal Relationships

,

14

, 463–480.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407597144003

Perlman, M., Siddiqui, A., Ram, A., & Ross, H. S. (2000). An analysis of sources of power in children's conflict interactions. In R. S. L. Mills & S. Duck (Eds.),

The developmental psychology of personal relationships

(pp. 155–174). Chichester, United Kingdom: Wiley.

Ram, A., & Ross, H. (2008). ‘We got to figure it out’: Information-sharing and siblings’ negotiations of conflicts of interests.

Social Development

,

17

, 512–527.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00436.x

Ross, H. S. (1996). Negotiating principles of entitlement in sibling property disputes.

Developmental Psychology

,

32

, 90–101.

https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.32.1.90

Siddiqui, A. A., & Ross, H. S. (1999). How do sibling conflicts end?

Early Education and Development

,

10

, 315–332.

https://doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed1003_5

Tucker, C. J., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C. (2008). Links between older and younger adolescent siblings’ adjustment: The moderating role of shared activities.

International Journal of Behavioral Development

,

32

, 152–160.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025407087214

Tucker, C. J., Updegraff, K., & Baril, M. E. (2010). Who's the boss? Patterns of control in adolescents’ sibling relationships.

Family Relations

,

59

, 520–532.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3729.2010.00620.x

Updegraff, K. A., McHale, S. M., Killoren, S. E., & Rodríguez, S. A. (2011). Cultural variations in sibling relationships. In J. Caspi (Ed.),

Sibling development: Implications for mental health practitioners

(pp. 83–105). New York, NY: Springer.

Vandell, D. L., & Bailey, M. D. (1992). Conflicts between siblings. In C. U. Shantz & W. W. Hartup (Eds.),

Conflict in child and adolescent development

(pp. 242–269). New York, NY: Cambridge.

Whiteman, S. D., Bernard, J. M., & Jensen, A. C. (2011). Sibling influence in human development. In J. Caspi (Ed.),

Sibling development: Implications for mental health practitioners

(pp. 1–15). New York, NY: Springer.

 

 

 

Nicole Campione-Barr, Ph.D.

,

is an Associate Professor in the Department of Psychological Sciences at the University of Missouri. Her primary areas of research focus on parent-adolescent and adolescent sibling relationships and adolescent adjustment.

Della Porta, S. & Howe, N. (2017). Siblings' power and influence in polyadic family conflict during early childhood. In N. Campione‐Barr (Ed.), Power, control, and influence in sibling relationships across development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 156, 15–31.

2