The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 1 -  - E-Book

The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 1 E-Book

4,5
153,99 €

oder
-100%
Sammeln Sie Punkte in unserem Gutscheinprogramm und kaufen Sie E-Books und Hörbücher mit bis zu 100% Rabatt.
Mehr erfahren.
Beschreibung

In the past fifty years, scholars of human development have been moving from studying change in humans within sharply defined periods, to seeing many more of these phenomenon as more profitably studied over time and in relation to other processes. The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 1: Cognition, Biology, and Methods presents the study of human development conducted by the best scholars in the 21st century. Social workers, counselors and public health workers will receive coverage of of the biological and cognitive aspects of human change across the lifespan.

Sie lesen das E-Book in den Legimi-Apps auf:

Android
iOS
von Legimi
zertifizierten E-Readern

Seitenzahl: 3799

Veröffentlichungsjahr: 2010

Bewertungen
4,5 (18 Bewertungen)
13
1
4
0
0
Mehr Informationen
Mehr Informationen
Legimi prüft nicht, ob Rezensionen von Nutzern stammen, die den betreffenden Titel tatsächlich gekauft oder gelesen/gehört haben. Wir entfernen aber gefälschte Rezensionen.



Table of Contents
Title Page
Copyright Page
Preface
A BRIEF HISTORY OF THE LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVE
THE VIEW FROM THE HILLS
CONCLUSIONS
THE GOALS OF THIS HANDBOOK
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFERENCES
Contributors
CHAPTER 1 - Life-Span Development
LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENT: CONCEPTS AND ISSUES
THE CONCEPT OF DEVELOPMENT
RELATIONAL METATHEORY
RELATIONAL DEVELOPMENTAL SYSTEMS
AGE, LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENT, AND AGING
CONCLUDING REMARKS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 2 - Emphasizing Intraindividual Variability in the Study of Development ...
CHARACTER OF VARIATION
INTRAINDIVIDUAL VARIATION AND THE STUDY OF CHANGE PROCESSES
THREE KEY EMPHASES IN STUDYING DEVELOPMENTAL PROCESSES THROUGH INTRAINDIVIDUAL VARIABILITY
THE INDIVIDUAL AS THE UNIT OF ANALYSIS
MULTIVARIATE MEASUREMENT SCHEMES
SIMILARITIES IN INTRAINDIVIDUAL VARIABILITY PATTERNS
SOME FINAL METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES
DEVELOPMENTAL RESEARCH ISSUES AND QUESTIONS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 3 - What Life-Span Data Do We Really Need?
THE NEED FOR APPROXIMATIONS IN LIFE-SPAN RESEARCH
INCOMPLETE DATA AND THE STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODEL APPROACH
WHAT PEOPLE DO WE REALLY NEED?
WHAT SCALES DO WE REALLY NEED?
WHAT ITEMS DO WE REALLY NEED?
WHAT OCCASIONS DO WE REALLY NEED?
WHAT DYNAMICS DO WE REALLY NEED?
DYNAMIC EXAMPLES FROM THE BERKELEY-BRADWAY STUDY
WHAT LIFE-SPAN DATA DO WE REALLY NEED?
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 4 - Brain Development
BRAIN STRUCTURE
PATTERNS OF CHANGE
DEVELOPMENT OF PREFRONTAL CORTEX
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 5 - Biology, Evolution, and Psychological Development
WHAT IS PSYCHOLOGY?
A RELATIONAL DEVELOPMENTAL SYSTEMS APPROACH
BIOLOGICAL FACTORS IN BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT: EVOLUTION AND GENETICS
BIOLOGICAL FACTORS IN BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT: THE BRAIN AND NERVOUS SYSTEM
BIOLOGY AND DEVELOPMENT
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 6 - The Dynamic Development of Thinking, Feeling, and Acting over the ...
FOUNDATIONS OF AN INTEGRATIVE THEORY OF DYNAMIC DEVELOPMENT
DYNAMIC DEVELOPMENT OF INTEGRATIVE PSYCHOLOGICAL STRUCTURES
DEVELOPMENTAL CHANGES IN LIFE GOALS OVER THE ADULT YEARS
ADULT DEVELOPMENT THROUGH SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS
DEVELOPMENTAL THEORY AND RESEARCH: THINK GLOBALLY, ACT LOCALLY
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 7 - Structure and Process in Life-Span Cognitive Development
EVALUATING MODELS OF CHANGE
LANGUAGE REPRESENTATION AND PROCESS ACROSS THE LIFE SPAN
PROCESS CHANGES IN MEMORY AND EXECUTIVE CONTROL
LIFE-SPAN CHANGES IN REPRESENTATION AND COGNITIVE CONTROL
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 8 - Fluid Cognitive Abilities and General Intelligence
FLUID COGNITIVE ABILITIES AND GENERAL INTELLIGENCE
PSYCHOMETRIC APPROACH
DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACH
NEUROBIOLOGICAL MODEL
THE NEUROBIOLOGICAL MODEL AND DEVELOPMENT
THE NEUROBIOLOGICAL MODEL IN CONTEXT
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 9 - Memory Development across the Life Span
SETTING THE STAGE FOR THE DEVELOPMENT OF DELIBERATE MEMORY
A PERSPECTIVE ON DELIBERATE MEMORY
LEARNING TO BE STRATEGIC
MEMORY STRATEGIES IN THE ADULT YEARS
RECOLLECTION AND FAMILIARITY
FACTORS THAT CONTRIBUTE TO AGE-RELATED CHANGES IN PERFORMANCE
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 10 - The Development of Mental Processing
MODELS FOR MENTAL PROCESSING
A COMPREHENSIVE MODEL OF THE MENTAL ARCHITECTURE AND DEVELOPMENT
MAPPING THE DEVELOPING MIND—DEVELOPING BRAIN RELATIONS
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 11 - The Development of Representation and Concepts
REPRESENTATION
CONCEPTS
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 12 - Development of Deductive Reasoning across the Life Span
NATURE AND SCOPE OF DEDUCTIVE REASONING
DEVELOPMENTAL THEORIES OF DEDUCTIVE REASONING
LATE ADULT DEVELOPMENT AND DEDUCTIVE REASONING
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 13 - Development of Executive Function across the Life Span
DEVELOPMENTAL CHANGES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTION ACROSS THE LIFE SPAN
THEORETICAL MODELS OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION
AN INTEGRATED DEVELOPMENTAL MODEL OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE FOR REPRESENTATIONAL DEVELOPMENT IN WORKING MEMORY AND ...
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 14 - Language Development
UNIVERSAL GRAMMAR
EMERGENTISM
TIME FRAMES
MECHANISMS OF EMERGENCE
LEVELS OF LANGUAGE
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 15 - Self-Regulation
DEFINITIONS OF SELF-REGULATION
EMOTION AND COGNITION IN SELF-REGULATION
ORGANIZATION OF THE CHAPTER: THEORY, DEFINITIONS, MEASUREMENT, AND A LIFE-SPAN ...
THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS IN THE STUDY OF SELF-REGULATION
SELF-REGULATION IN MULTIPLE DISCIPLINES
INTEGRATION OF EMOTION AND COGNITION IN BEHAVIOR
MEASUREMENT
DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-REGULATION OVER THE LIFE SPAN
CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 16 - The Development of Morality
IS ALL THIS AN ILLUSION?
IS DEVELOPMENT A CONSEQUENCE OF ONLY ENVIRONMENT OR BIOLOGY (OR ADDITIONS OF ...
LIFE IS NOT EASY: DECISIONS ARE COMPLICATED AND TAKE INTO ACCOUNT MULTIPLE CONSIDERATIONS
ANOTHER WAY OF THINKING ABOUT EMOTIONS AND THINKING
ALL IS NOT JUST ACCEPTED: OPPOSITION AND MORAL RESISTANCE
MORALITY IN ADULTHOOD
PEOPLE REALLY DO THINK, PLAN, AND CHOOSE
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 17 - The Development of Social Understanding
SOCIAL COGNITION AS “THEORY OF MIND”: EVIDENCE ACROSS THE LIFE COURSE
THEORIES OF “THEORY OF MIND”: THE QUEST FOR A MIND DETACHED FROM SOCIAL PROCESSES
CURRENT STATE OF PLAY: MOVES TO INCREASED INDIVIDUALIZATION
INTERPERSONAL INTERACTION AND SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING
DEVELOPMENT OF SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING IN INFANCY
LANGUAGE AND SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 18 - The Emergence of Consciousness and Its Role in Human Development
INTRODUCTION
INTENTION IS NOT INTENTIONALITY
RELATION OF COGNITION AND EMOTION
ROLE OF THE KNOWER IN KNOWING
WHAT IS AND IS NOT A SELF
THE SELF SYSTEM
HISTORICAL VIEWS OF SELF-DEVELOPMENT: DUALITY OF THE SELF
WHAT A SELF IS AND HOW I KNOW I KNOW
CONSCIOUSNESS: THE CORE PROCESSES OF SELF AND THE MENTAL STATE OF ME
MEASUREMENT OF EXPLICIT CONSCIOUSNESS
ROLE OF EXPLICIT CONSCIOUSNESS IN DEVELOPMENT
SOME WORDS ON THE IMPLICIT-EXPLICIT PREDICAMENT
LIFE SPAN OF CONSCIOUSNESS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 19 - The Development of Knowing
BELIEFS AND BELIEFS ABOUT BELIEFS
BALKANIZATION AND THE COLLAPSE OF THE ONTOGENETIC EMPIRE
SHORT HISTORICAL SKETCH
FINDING SOME FOLLOWABLE PATH THROUGH ALL OF THE INCOMMENSURABLE THINGS COMMONLY ...
PART I: CHILDREN’S DEVELOPING THEORIES OF MIND—A PRIMER
PART II: IS THERE METAREPRESENTATIONAL LIFE BEFORE AGE 4?
PART IIIA: IS THERE EPISTEMIC DEVELOPMENT AFTER AGE 4?
PART IIIB: EPISTEMIC DEVELOPMENT BEYOND THE CHILDHOOD YEARS
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 20 - Spatial Development
MAJOR THEMES IN THE STUDY OF LIFE-SPAN SPATIAL DEVELOPMENT
SPATIAL DEVELOPMENT DURING CHILDHOOD
SPATIAL REASONING IN ADULTS
INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN SPATIAL COGNITION
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 21 - Gesturing across the Life Span
SITUATING GESTURE IN RELATION TO NONVERBAL BEHAVIOR AND SIGN LANGUAGE
GESTURE IS A WINDOW TO THE MIND
ROLE OF GESTURE IN COMMUNICATION OVER THE LIFE SPAN
ROLE OF GESTURE IN COGNITION OVER THE LIFE SPAN
GESTURING EARLY IN DEVELOPMENT
GESTURING AND LATE ADULT DEVELOPMENT
WHAT GESTURE TELLS US ABOUT COMMUNICATION DISORDERS OVER THE LIFE SPAN
GESTURING ACROSS THE LIFE SPAN
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 22 - Developmental Psychopathology—Self, Embodiment, Meaning
WHAT THE CONCEPT OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY MEANS IN THE GENERAL FIELD OF PSYCHOLOGY
A HOLISTIC RELATIONAL APPROACH TO DEFINING THE CONCEPT OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY
A CONCEPTUAL MATRIX ADDRESSING DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY FROM THE ...
THE FIRST PROBE: THE FOUNDATION AND FIRST FLOOR OF THE INNER SELF: EMBODIED ...
THE SECOND PROBE: COGNITIVE-EMOTIONAL REGULATORS AND INSTRUMENTAL-EXPRESSIVE BEHAVIORS
THE THIRD PROBE: EXPLORING RELATIONS AMONG ENVIRONMENTS, ...
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 23 - The Meaning of Wisdom and Its Development Throughout Life
HISTORICAL CONCEPTIONS OF WISDOM
PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH ABOUT THE MEANING OF WISDOM
DEVELOPMENT OF WISDOM
REFERENCES
CHAPTER 24 - Thriving across the Life Span
CONCEPT OF THRIVING IN DEVELOPMENTAL STUDY
DEFINITION AND PRINCIPLES OF THRIVING
REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE RELATED TO THRIVING THROUGHOUT THE LIFE SPAN
TOWARD A SCIENTIFIC INVESTIGATION OF THRIVING
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
THE HANDBOOK OF LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENT:
Author Index
Subject Index
This book is printed on acid-free paper.
Copyright © 2010 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved.
Published by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey. Published simultaneously in Canada.
No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 646-8600, or on the web at www.copyright.com. Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008.
Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials. The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a professional where appropriate. Neither the publisher nor author shall be liable for any loss of profit or any other commercial damages, including but not limited to special, incidental, consequential, or other damages.
This publication is designed to provide accurate and authoritative information in regard to the subject matter covered. It is sold with the understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering professional services. If legal, accounting, medical, psychological or any other expert assistance is required, the services of a competent professional person should be sought.
Designations used by companies to distinguish their products are often claimed as trademarks. In all instances where John Wiley & Sons, Inc. is aware of a claim, the product names appear in initial capital or all capital letters. Readers, however, should contact the appropriate companies for more complete information regarding trademarks and registration.
For general information on our other products and services please contact our Customer Care Department within the U.S. at (800) 762-2974, outside the United States at (317) 572-3993 or fax (317) 572-4002.
Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats. Some content that appears in print may not be available in electronic books. For more information about Wiley products, visit our website at www.wiley.com.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data:
The handbook of life-span development / editor-in-chief, Richard M. Lerner.
p. cm.
Includes author and subject indexes.
ISBN 978-0-470-39011-5 (v. 1 : cloth); ISBN 978-0-470-39012-2 (v. 2 : cloth); ISBN 978-0-470-39013-9 (set : cloth); 978-0-470-63433-2 (ebk); 978-0-470-63434-9 (ebk); 978-0-470-63435-6 (ebk)
1. Developmental psychology. 2. Maturation (Psychology) 3. Aging-Psychological aspects. I. Lerner, Richard M.
BF713.H3648 2010
155-dc22
2009049300
Preface
Until the early 1960s, the field of human development was dominated by either descriptions of the behavioral or psychological phenomena presumptively unfolding as a consequence of genetically controlled timetables of maturational change (e.g., see the chapters by Hess and by McClearn in the third edition of the Handbook of Child Psychology; Mussen, 1970) or by descriptions of the behaviors presumptively elicited in response to stimulation encountered over the course of early life experiences (e.g., see the chapters by Stevenson or by White in the same edition of the Handbook). Framed within a Cartesian dualism that split nature from nurture (Overton, 2006), developmental science focused in the main on the generic human being (Emmerich, 1968) and on the earliest years of life or, at most, the years surrounding the stages of pubertal change. These periods were regarded as the portions of ontogeny in which the fundamental processes of human development emerged and functioned to shape the subsequent course of human life (Brim & Kagan, 1980).
Today, the study of development has evolved from a field embedded within the domain of developmental psychology to an area of scholarship labeled developmental science (Bornstein & Lamb, 2005, 2010; Magnusson & Stattin, 1998, 2006). Substantively, developmental science is a field that conceptualizes the entire span of human life as potentially involving developmental change. The possibility of developmental change across life exists because the basic process of development is seen as involving mutually influential relations between an active organism and a changing, multilevel ecology, a relation represented as individual ↔ context relations (Lerner, 2006). These relations provide the fundamental impetus to systematic and successive changes across the life span (Brandtstädter, 1998; Overton, 1973, 2003; Lerner, 2006).
Thus, the contemporary study of human development involves placing postmodern, relational models at the cutting edge of theoretical and empirical interest (Overton, 2006). These models consider all levels of organization—from the inner biological through the physical ecological, cultural, and historical—as involved in mutually influential relationships across the breadth of the entire life course (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006; Riegel, 1975, 1976). Variations in time and place constitute vital sources of systematic changes across ontogeny—even into the 10th and 11th decades of life—and, as such, human life is variegated and characterized by intraindividual change and interindividual differences (Baltes, Lindenberger, & Staudinger, 2006; Elder, Modell, & Parke, 1993). Accordingly, because ontogenetic change is embodied in its relation to time and place (Overton, 2006), contemporary developmental science regards the temporality represented by historical changes as imbued in all levels of organization, as coacting integratively, and as providing a potential for this systematic change—for plasticity—across the life span.
In short, as a consequence of the relational coactions of changes at levels of organization ranging from the biological and the psychological and behavioral to the sociocultural, designed and natural physical ecological, and through the historical (see Gottlieb, 1997; Overton, 2006), processes of development are viewed in contemporary developmental science through a theoretical and empirical lens that extends the study of change across the human ontogenetic span and, as well, through generational and historical time (Elder, 1998; Elder et al., 1993). The variations in the actions of individuals on their contexts and contexts on individuals integratively propel and texture the course of life (Baltes, Freund, & Li, 2005; Brandtstädter, 2006; Freund & Baltes, 2000 Freund, Li, & Baltes, 1999). As a result, the breadth of the life span and all levels of organization within the ecology of human development must be considered to fully describe, explain, and optimize the course of intraindividual change and of interindividual differences in such change (Baltes et al., 2006; Baltes, Reese, & Nesselroade, 1977).
There exist both historical (Baltes, 1979,1983; Cairns & Cairns, 2006), philosophical, and theoretical (Lerner, 1984; Overton, 1973, 1975, 2006) accounts of the nature and bases of the evolution of developmental science. These accounts document that the field changed from one dominated by psychological, environmental, or biological reductionist, split, and age-period-restricted conceptions of human development processes to become a field focused on relational, systems, and life-span developmental models. As Edwin G. Boring (1950, p. ix) noted, Hermann Ebbinghaus once remarked that “psychology has a long past, but only a short history.” In many ways, the same statement may be made about the evolution of the life-span view of human development.

A BRIEF HISTORY OF THE LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVE

The life-span ideas that we today summarize as “the” lifespan perspective began to emerge in the United States in the mid-1960s and early 1970s. However, the conception that was forwarded was neither a newly created perspective of human development nor the only extant conception of lifespan development present at the time (or even now). To a great extent, the history of the emergence and refinement of this life-span perspective arose through the discussions at, and the subsequent edited volumes derived from, a series of conferences held in the late 1960s and early to mid-1970s at the University of West Virginia in Morgantown. The conferences were held under the aegis of the Department of Psychology, which was chaired by K. Warner Schaie. As Baltes (1979, 1983) and Cairns and Cairns (2006) explained, the roots of the approach that began to crystallize in Morgantown can be traced to 19th-century scholarship in the United States and to philosophical ideas forwarded about 200 years earlier in Europe. In turn, although there was a different concentration of empirical attention paid to successive portions of human ontogeny (with most attention paid to infancy and childhood and comparatively less work devoted to studying people as they aged into the adolescent and adult periods), many U.S. developmental psychologists might argue that the study of human life within the social and behavioral sciences has always included a focus on behavior across the breadth of ontogeny. Despite the fact that the label life-span developmental psychology was not used, there was, at least since G. Stanley Hall’s work on senescence (1922), attention paid in some way to periods of life beyond childhood and adolescence.
Nevertheless, despite any claims made that developmental science has been concerned for at least 90 years with development across life, the structure and function of academic work would contradict such assertions. Indeed, it was not until the 1970s and early 1980s, when the term life-span developmental psychology became popular in developmental psychology (e.g., Baltes & Schaie, 1973; Goulet & Baltes, 1970; Nesselroade & Reese, 1973), that many departments began offering life-span development courses.
Moreover, most people involved in teaching these courses were trained in infancy, childhood, or, in a few cases, adolescence. They taught what they knew most and, as such, courses were mostly modestly revised child development courses with a few lectures at the end of the course devoted to “adult development and aging.” The textbooks that were written for use in these courses—in the main by colleagues also not involved in the study of developmental change across the breadth of human ontogeny—reinforced the approach taken by classroom instructors. Texts were in fact slightly revised child development books with a couple of chapters (and in some cases only one chapter) added about adult development and aging; at the time, the late adult years were still not seen as a part of the process of development. In short, authors wrote these books, and publishers structured them, to meet the “needs” of the instructors, which were to present mostly infancy and childhood, perhaps a chapter on adolescence (puberty, the identity crisis, and problem behaviors were in large part the focus of such chapters) and, finally, “adulthood and aging.”
When the work of the key scholars studying the latter portion of the life span, for instance, developmental scientists such as Paul B. Baltes, James Birren, John R. Nesselroade, or K. Warner Schaie, among others, were cited, this scholarship was reduced to the fact that these scholars had promoted the idea of studying development across the life span. Therefore, because the existence of such life-span work required some treatment in the textbook, the author of the textbook would commence to present such topics as the life tasks of adulthood (as discussed, for instance, by Robert Havighurst, 1951), some ideas about intellectual decline with aging, and “death and dying.” What was wrong with such approaches to life-span human development?

THE VIEW FROM THE HILLS

These early textbooks in life-span development missed the points being made by the scholars who were gathering in Morgantown, West Virginia, and who were shaping quite a different approach to the study of development across the life span. The work of Baltes, Schaie, Nesselroade, and other major contributors (e.g., scholars such as Nancy Datan, Lawrence Goulet, Willis Overton, Hayne Reese, and Klaus Riegel) to the foundation of the approach to life-span development that has evolved to frame developmental science had, at best, been misinterpreted and, at worst (and in fact most of the time), trivialized. What, then, were the ideas being developed by the scholars meeting in Morgantown? Answering this question is central to the present work: The ideas that began to be developed in the West Virginia hills provide the foundation of the scholarship represented in this Handbook. Indeed, one cannot overestimate the impact on developmental science theory and methodology of the books that were derived from the West Virginia conferences (Baltes & Schaie, 1973; Datan & Reese, 1977; Goulet & Baltes, 1970; Nesselroade & Reese, 1973) and from a subsequent series of “annual” advances volumes, Life-Span Development and Behavior , first edited by Paul Baltes (1978); then by Baltes and Orville G. Brim, Jr. (1979, 1980, 1982, 1983, 1984); by Baltes, David L. Featherman, and Richard M. Lerner (1986, 1988a, 1988b,1990); and by Featherman, Lerner, and Marion Perlmutter (1992, 1994). These works pushed the study of human development beyond the split and reductionist conceptual boundaries to which the field had accommodated (Overton, 2006).
As Baltes (1987; Baltes et al., 2006) explained, the lifespan view of human development was associated with the integration of a set of ideas, each of which could be found as “stand-alone” concepts within the developmental literature; however, when taken as an integrated or, in fact, a fused, or relational, whole, these ideas changed the conceptual landscape of the field. As such, the set of concepts introduced in the late 1960s and early 1970s at the University of West Virginia life-span development conferences embodied an approach to the study of the human life span that stood in sharp contrast to the simplistic, additive approach to development found in the early textbooks and associated courses in life-span development. Although these “West Virginian” ideas have of course evolved across the ensuing decades and were refined and extended by the their originators and by those who were influenced or trained by them, the fundamental character of these ideas remains the same and constitutes at least a sea change, if not a true paradigm shift, in the nature of thinking about human development.
First, development was seen as a process, one that began at conception and continued through the end of life. For example, developmental processes were conceptualized as involving systematic and successive changes in the organization of relations within and across the levels of organization comprising the ecology of human development (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006; Lerner, 2002, 2006; Overton, 1973, 1978, 2006; Overton & Reese, 1981; Reese & Overton, 1970). There were both qualitatively and quantitatively continuous and discontinuous facets of this process. Accordingly, mutidirectionality of development (increases, decreases, curvilinearity, smooth or abrupt change, etc.) were all possible forms for a developmental process, and the shape or form of a developmental trajectory for an individual or group was a matter of theory-predicated empirical inquiry (Wohlwill, 1973).
In addition, because variation in the form of developmental trajectories may occur for different people (e.g., people who vary with regard to age, sex, race, birth cohort, etc.), living in different settings, or in different historical eras, developmental process may take a different form at different points in ontogeny, generational time, or history across individuals or groups. Thus, diversity—with regard to within-person changes but also to differences between people in within-person change—rose to the level of substantive significance (as opposed to error variance) within the life-span view. For instance, as explained by Bornstein (1995), in regard to his “specificity principle” of infant development “specific experiences at specific times exert specific effects over specific aspects of infant growth in specific ways” (p. 21). In turn, a similar idea was advanced by Freund, Nikitin, and Ritter (2009), albeit one focused at the other end of the life span. Underscoring the importance of viewing the developmental process across the breadth of ontogeny, Freund et al. noted that a person’s development during a historical period of extended life expectancy is likely to have important implications for development during young and middle adulthood.
Accordingly, although it would make sense from a lifespan developmental perspective to study individuals and groups within (as well as across) ontogenetic “age periods,” an age period-specific focus should not be adopted because of the mistaken belief that the developmental process that occurs in childhood or adolescence is somehow a different developmental process than the one that occurs in adulthood and late adulthood. Rather, the life-span developmental scientist has the task of describing and explaining and, as noted again later in this preface, optimizing the form of such change across life. He or she must detail the ways in which changes within one period are derived from changes at earlier periods and affect changes at subsequent periods.
As well, explanations of continuities and discontinuities across life, and of the form (the shape) of the developmental trajectory and of its rate of change, involve a very different theoretical frame than the ones that had been dominant in other approaches to the study of development (i.e., the split and reductionist approaches of past eras). As explained by Overton, both in his prior work (e.g., 1973, 2006) and in the chapter that introduces Volume 1 of the present Handbook, a relational metamodel frames the contemporary, cutting-edge study of human development within and across all portions of ontogeny. The use of this relational perspective emerged through the influence of Overton and other developmental scientists (e.g., Sameroff, 1975), including those studying infancy (e.g., Bornstein, 1995; Lamb, 1977; Lewis, 1972; Lewis & Rosenblum, 1974; Thelen & Smith, 1998), as well as through the contributions of comparative scientists (e.g., Gottlieb, 1997; Greenberg & Tobach, 1984; Kuo, 1976; Schneirla, 1957) and biologists (e.g., Novikoff, 1945a, 1945b; von Bertalanffy, 1933, 1968). This scholarship resulted in the elaboration of several variants of a developmental systems theoretical model of development (e.g., Ford & Lerner, 1992; Lerner, 2002); more recently, by explicitly incorporating a relational perspective, these models have been termed relational developmental systems models of development (Lerner, 2006; Lerner & Overton, 2008).
Because it may seem counterintuitive that the scholarship of developmental scientists studying infancy was integral in the foundation of the life-span approach to human development, it is useful to illustrate briefly the contributions of such work to the life-span approach. The scholarship of Michael Lewis (e.g., 1972; Lewis & Lee-Painter, 1974) built on the insights of Bell (1968), about the potential presence in correlational data about socialization, of evidence for the bidirectional influences between parents and children. Lewis and his colleagues launched a program of research that provided a new, relational model of infant-parent interaction. In The Effect of the Infant on Its Caregiver (Lewis & Rosenblum, 1974), a volume that represents a watershed event in the history of the study of human development through the use of person↔context relational models, Lewis and colleagues argued that “Not only is the infant or child influenced by its social, political, economic and biological world, but in fact the child itself influences its world in turn” (Lewis & Rosenblum, 1974, p. xv) and maintained that “only through interaction can we study, without distortion, human behavior (Lewis & Lee-Painter, 1974, p. 21). Envisioning the relational, developmental systems models that would come to the fore in the study of human development a quarter century later, Lewis noted that a
relational position not only requires that we deal with elements in interaction but also requires that we not consider the static quality of these interactions. Rather, it is necessary to study their flow with time….Exactly how this might be done is not at all clear. It may be necessary to consider a more metaphysical model, a circle in which there are neither elements nor beginnings/ends. (Lewis & Lee-Painter, 1974, pp. 46-47).
Lewis’s scholarship fostered an intellectual climate among other infancy researchers that resulted in a reconceptualization of phenomena of infant development within the sorts of relational, individual ↔ context relational models he forwarded. I noted earlier in this preface the contributions of Marc Bornstein (1995) in this regard. Here, too, however, we should point out another, foundational instance of the contributions of infancy researchers to the life-span perspective, found in the work of Michael Lamb. For instance, Lamb and his colleagues (e.g., Lamb, 1977; Lamb, Thompson, Gardner, & Charnov, 1985; Thompson & Lamb, 1986) approached the study of infant attachment within the context of the assumptions that (a) children influence their “socializers” and are not simply the receptive foci of socializing forces, (b) early social and emotional/personality development occurs in the context of a complex family system rather than only in the context of the mother-infant dyad, and (c) social and psychological development are not confined to infancy or childhood but involve a process that continues from birth to death (Lamb, 1978, p. 137; cf. Riley, 1979).
Within this conceptual framework, Lamb and his colleagues found that prior interpretations of infant attachment, which included “an emphasis on the formative significance of early experiences, a focus on unidirectional influences on the child, a tendency to view development within a narrow ecological context, and a search for universal processes of developmental change” (Thompson & Lamb, 1986, p. 1), were less powerful in accounting for the findings of attachment research than an interpretation associated with person↔context relational models. Accordingly, in a review of attachment research conducted through the mid-1980s, Lamb and colleagues concluded that “reciprocal organism-environment influences, developmental plasticity, individual patterns of developmental change and broader contextual influences on development can better help to integrate and interpret the attachment literature, and may also provide new directions for study” (Thompson & Lamb, 1986, p. 1).
Lamb’s work challenged the field of infancy to move the study of the early years of life beyond the use of narrow, split, or reductionist conceptions of the exclusive influences of heredity or early experiences or of simplistic views of proximal dyadic relationships acting in isolation from the fuller and richer ecology of human development. He provided instead a relational vision for the understanding of infancy as part of the entire life span, of the life-span development of the other people in the infant’s world, and of the complex set of infant ↔ social context relations as reciprocal exchanges in and with a multilevel and dynamic context (e.g., see Lamb, 1977; Lamb et al., 1985).
In short, as exemplified by the work embedding the study of infancy within a life-span approach to human development, the theoretical approaches that emerged in the 1970s from the impetus given developmental science by the scholars gathering in the hills of West Virginia constituted an integrative approach to variables across the levels of organization within the relational developmental system. As such, these models rejected as counterfactual the split conceptions, theories, and metatheories that partitioned the sources of development into nature and nurture. Rejected as well were “compromise” views that, although admitting that both of these purportedly separate sources of influence were involved in development, used problematic (i.e., additive) conceptions of interaction (conceptualized much as are interaction terms in an analysis of variance or in other instantiations of the general linear model; see Gottlieb, Wahlsten, & Lickliter, 2006). Instead, the theoretical models promoted by the scholars contributing to the foundation and evolution of the life-span perspective stressed that the basic process of human development involves developmental regulations—that is, individual ↔ context relations that link all levels of the ecology of human development within a thoroughly integrated, or “fused” (Tobach & Greenberg, 1984), dynamic, relational system.
For instance, these levels of the ecology involve ecological systems within the person (i.e., biosocial influences) or most proximal to him or her (what Bronfenbrenner [1979] termed the microsystem), extend to the set of contexts within which the individual interacts (the mesosystem; Bronfenbrenner, 1979) and to the systems in the ecology within which components of the mesosystem (e.g., parents) interact (e.g., the workplace) that may not directly involve the person but nevertheless may have an impact on him or her. In addition, the ecology includes the macrosystem—the broadest level of the ecology; the macrosystem influences all other systems and includes social policies, major intuitions of society (such as education, health care, and the economy), the designed and natural physical ecology, and, ultimately, history. As noted, this latter level of organization within the relational, integrated developmental system provides a temporal component for all other facets of the developmental system and creates the potential for systematic change, for plasticity, in individual ↔ context relations.
One recent instantiation of such relational, developmental systems thinking in regard to the dynamics of the person’s exchanges with his or her ecology involves the work of Freund and colleagues in regard to the nature of the goals that individuals pursue within their changing context (e.g., Freund, 2007; Nikitin & Freund, 2008). Freund argues that the interplay of different levels of organization is important not only at the levels of person, family, and society (or other nested levels) but also for understanding the role of specific constructs—in this case, goals—for development. Extending action-theoretical concepts that have tended to view goals as primarily personal constructs, Freund explained that goals are located at multiple levels of the developmental system and that mutually influential relations among these levels need to be assessed to understand fully the nature and role of goals in human development. These levels involve social norms and expectations that inform about age-related opportunity structures and goal-relevant resources; personal beliefs about the appropriate timing and sequencing of goals; personal goals that are influenced by social norms, personal beliefs, the individual’s learning history, and external (e.g., social and physical environment) and internal (e.g., talent) resources; and nonconscious goals and motives that might be particularly influential in times of transition or in times of routine.

CONCLUSIONS

A relational developmental systems model of human development constitutes the approach to studying the life span that evolved from the ideas developed among the scholars gathering in the formative conferences at the University of West Virginia (Baltes & Schaie, 1973; Datan & Reese, 1977; Goulet & Baltes, 1970; Nesselroade & Reese, 1973). It is this dynamic, relational perspective that constitutes a radical departure from approaches that study nature, or nurture, or additive, linear (or even curvilinear) combinations (even if cast as “interactions”) between the two. Because of the emphasis on the dynamics of relations between the multiple levels of organization involved in the individual and the multiple levels of organization that are part of the ecology of human development, we need to adopt an ontogenetically all-encompassing—a fully life-span—approach to studying relational developmental processes. Such a conceptual frame is required if we are to understand the import of individual ↔ context relations for fostering specific changes, for a specific individual or group, living in a specific context, at a specific point in history (Bornstein, 1995). Clearly, such scholarship requires a multidisciplinary integrative (truly interdisciplinary) approach to the study of human development. Indeed, this need for collaboration across disciplines is in large part why developmental psychology has been transformed into developmental science (Lerner, 2006).
In addition, the concepts of developmental regulation and plasticity integral to a developmental systems framing of the study of development across the life span have another quite important implication for the conduct of developmental science. These concepts in combination afford optimism about the possibility of finding or instantiating individual ↔ context relations that may increase the probability of positive developmental change. Indeed, the life-span approach to developmental science suggests that applications of relational, theory-predicated research findings (in the form of policies or programs) may promote more positive courses of development across life. Efforts to optimize the course of human life through the application of relationally framed developmental science provide, then, an opportunity to test developmental systems ideas about the impact of changes in individual ↔ context relations. As well, such applied efforts constitute a way for developmental scientists to contribute to the improvement of human life among diverse people and their settings. Ultimately, then, such applications of developmental science may contribute to the enhancement of social justice (Lerner & Overton, 2008).

THE GOALS OF THISHANDBOOK

The innovative ideas associated with a developmental systems-framed approach to the study of life-span development is coupled with an admittedly ambitious agenda of basic and applied developmental science devoted to studying and optimizing processes of individual ↔ context relations across the life span. Nevertheless, despite the methodological complexities of adopting this relational approach to developmental science, the past 40 years have provided depth and breadth of empirical evidence in support of the usefulness of these ideas in framing methodologically rigorous and substantively significant developmental science.
As such, one might think that numerous scholarly references exist for scholars to draw on to understand the state-of-the art of the study of life-span development. However, such resources have not existed before the present Handbook. Because of this absence, there was no single reference work that developmental scientists or their students could consult to find a thorough, integrative presentation of the breadth of scholarship documenting the use of the relational, developmental systems ideas that frame the life-span study of human development. There was no single high-level reference that provided discussions of the usefulness of relational concepts in integrating and extending the range of substantive areas involved in studying development across the life span.
Instead, to date, the key reference works available to developmental scientists and their students about the nature and scope of life-span change processes have been—paradoxically—age-segmented resources (e.g., Damon & Lerner, 2006; Lerner & Steinberg, 2009). In short, despite the important and rich theoretical and substantive work that is framed by perspectives on human development that encompass the life span, there has been no single reference work that presents the top-tier developmental science work pertinent to such processes.
The goal of this Handbook is to provide such a scholarly resource. It is the first-ever reference work to present—through the top-tier scholars in developmental science—the accumulated knowledge about the description, explanation, and implications for optimizing applications (i.e., applications that have the potential to maximize the chances for positive human development) of development across the life span. My fellow editors and I have the aspiration that this Handbook will constitute a watershed event in the development of life-span developmental scholarship. With the publication of this Handbook, we believe a compelling scholarly alternative will exist to counter both split depictions of developmental processes (e.g., studying childhood and adulthood as if they were composed of distinct, completely discontinuous processes) and split explanations of the changes that occur across the life span.
We hope that this first edition of the Handbook will serve as a touchstone for current and future researchers and instructors. As such, future editions of the Handbook may provide an even richer depiction of the course of development across the breadth of the life span than is possible in this edition. Given that developmental science has had for so long an age-specific focus, several topics discussed across the two volumes of this Handbook remain underexplored, perhaps particularly with respect to the later adult years. When the literature was not available to discuss a particular topic in depth with regard to a portion of the life span, the authors point to this situation and suggest ways to expand the topic more fully across the life span. We believe that their ideas for future scholarship are persuasive—indeed, compelling. As such, our ultimate hope is that future editions of the Handbook will reflect the continued theoretical, methodological, and empirical refinement of the concepts about development that began to coalesce in the hills of West Virginia more than four decades ago.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

There are numerous people to whom the editors of this Handbook owe enormous thanks for their contributions. Clearly, we are deeply grateful to the colleagues who contributed to this work, both for their superb scholarly contributions and for their commitment to working collaboratively to produce this Handbook. We also appreciate greatly the unflagging support of our superb editor at John Wiley & Sons, Patricia Rossi. Her commitment to the vision of this Handbook and her support for the quality of contribution we sought to make to developmental science were essential, indeed invaluable, assets throughout our work. We are also grateful to Leslie Dickinson and Jarrett M. Lerner, successive Managing Editors at the Institute for Applied Research at Tufts University, for their superb editorial work. Their commitment to quality and productivity, and their resiliency in the face of the tribulations of manuscript production, are greatly admired and deeply appreciated. I am also grateful to the National 4-H Council, the Philip Morris Smoking Prevention Department, the John Templeton Foundation, the Thrive Foundation for Youth, and the National Science Foundation for supporting my work during the development of this project.
Finally, my co-editors and I dedicate this Handbook to Paul B. Baltes, one of the pillars of 20th century developmental science and, across the last third of the 20th century, the key intellectual and professional force involved in establishing and enabling the flourishing of theory and research about life-span development. His intellect, leadership, generosity, kindness, and wisdom are warmly remembered and sorely missed.
Richard M. Lerner Medford, MA October 1, 2009

REFERENCES

Baltes, P. B. (Ed.). (1978). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.
Baltes, P. B. (1979). On the potential and limits of child development: Life-span developmental perspectives. Newsletter of the Society of Research in Child Development, 1-4.
Baltes, P. B. (1983). Life-span developmental psychology: Observations on history and theory revisited. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Developmental psychology: Historical and philosophical perspectives (pp. 79-111). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611-626.
Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. (Eds.). (1979). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press.
Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. (Eds.). (1980). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 3). New York: Academic Press.
Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. (Eds.). (1982). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 4). New York: Academic Press.
Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. (Eds.). (1983). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 5). New York: Academic Press.
Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. (Eds.). (1984). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 6). New York: Academic Press.
Baltes, P. B., Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1986). Lifespan development and behavior (Vol. 7). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baltes, P. B., Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1988a). Lifespan development and behavior (Vol. 8). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baltes, P. B., Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1988b). Lifespan development and behavior (Vol. 9). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baltes, P. B., Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1990). Lifespan development and behavior (Vol. 10). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baltes, P. B., Freund, A.M., & Li, S.-C. (2005). The psychological science of human aging. In M. Johnson, V. L. Bengston, P. G. Coleman, & T. B. L. Kirkwood (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of age and ageing (pp. 47-71). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Lifespan theory in developmental psychology. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.) Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 569-664). Editors-in-chief: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1977). Life-span developmental psychology: Introduction to research methods. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Baltes, P. B., & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.). (1973). Life-span developmental psychology: Personality and socialization. New York: Academic Press.
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75, 81-95.
Boring, E. G. (1950). A history of experimental psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Bornstein, M. H. (1995). Parenting infants. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting (Vol. 1, pp. 3-39). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bornstein, M. H., & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.). (2005). Developmental science: An advanced textbook (5th ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bornstein, M. H., & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.). (2010). Developmental science: An advanced textbook (6th ed.). New York: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.
Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Action perspectives on human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 807-863). Editor in chief: W. Damon. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Brandtstädter, J. (2006). Action perspectives on human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 516-568). Editors-in-chief: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Brim, O. G., & Kagan, J. (Eds.). (1980). Constancy and change in human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 793-828). Editor in chief: W. Damon. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (2006). The making of developmental psychology. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 89- 165). Editors-in-chief: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Editors-in-Chief). (2006). Handbook of child psychology (6th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Datan, N., & Reese, H. W. (Eds.). (1977). Life-span developmental psychology: Dialectical perspectives on experimental research. New York: Academic Press.
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1998). The life course and human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 939-991). Editor in chief: W. Damon. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Elder, G. H., Modell, J., & Parke, R. D. (Eds.). (1993). Children in time and place: Developmental and historical insights. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Emmerich, W. (1968). Personality development and concepts of structure. Child Development, 39, 671-690.
Featherman, D. L., Lerner, R. M., & Perlmutter, M. (Eds.). (1992). Lifespan development and behavior (Vol. 11). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Featherman, D. L., Lerner, R. M., & Perlmutter, M. (Eds.). (1994). Lifespan development and behavior (Vol. 12). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ford, D. H., & Lerner, R. M. (1992). Developmental systems theory: An integrative approach. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Freund, A. M. (2007). Differentiating and integrating levels of goal representation: A life-span perspective. In B. R. Little, K. Salmela-Aro, J. E. Nurmi, & S. D. Phillips (Eds.), Personal project pursuit: Goals, action and human flourishing (pp. 247-270). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). The orchestration of selection, optimization, and compensation: An action-theoretical conceptualization of a theory of developmental regulation. In W. J. Perrig & A. Grob (Eds.), Control of human behaviour, mental processes and consciousness (pp. 35-58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Freund, A. M., Li, K. Z. H., & Baltes, P. B. (1999). The role of selection, optimization, and compensation in successful aging. In J. Brandtstädter & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Action and development: Origins and functions of intentional self-development (pp. 401-434). Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Freund, A. M., Nikitin, J., & Ritter, J. O. (2009). Psychological consequences of longevity: The increasing importance of self-regulation in old age. Human Development, 52, 1- 37.
Gottlieb, G. (1997). Synthesizing nature-nurture: Prenatal roots of instinctive behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gottlieb, G., Wahlsten, D., & Lickliter, R. (2006). The significance of biology for human development: A developmental psychobiological systems view. In R. M. Lerner & W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 210-257). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Goulet, L. R., & Baltes, P. B. (Eds.). (1970). Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory. New York: Academic Press.
Greenberg, G., & Tobach, E. (Eds.) Behavioral evolution and integrative levels. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hall, G. S. (1922). Senescence: The last half of life. New York: Appleton. Havighurst, R. J. (1951). Developmental tasks and education. New York: Longmans.
Hess, E. H. (1970). Ethology and developmental psychology. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 1-38). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Kuo, Z.-Y. (1976). The dynamics of behavior development: An epigenetic view. New York: Plenum.
Lamb, M. E. (1977). A reexamination of the infant social world. Human Development, 20, 65-85.
Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W. P., & Charnov, E. L. (1985). Infant-mother attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lerner, R. M. (2002). Concepts and theories of human development (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lerner, R. M. (2006). Developmental science, developmental systems, and contemporary theories of human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 1-17). Editors-in-chief: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Lerner, R. M., & Overton, W. F. (2008). Exemplifying the integrations of the relational developmental system: Synthesizing theory, research, and application to promote positive development and social justice. Journal of Adolescent Research, 23, 245-255.
Lerner, R. M., & Steinberg, L. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Lewis, M. (1972). State as an infant-environment interaction: An analysis of mother infant behavior as a function of sex. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 95-121.
Lewis, M., & Lee-Painter, S. (1974). An interactional approach to the mother-infant dyad. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregivers (pp. 21-48). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Lewis, M., & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.). (1974). The effect of the infant on its caregivers. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Magnusson, D., & Stattin, H. (1998). Person-context interaction theories. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 685- 759). Editor in chief: W. Damon. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Magnusson, D., & Stattin, H. (2006). The person in context: A holistic-interactionistic approach. In R. M. Lerner & W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 400-464). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
McClearn, G. R. (1970). Genetic influences on behavior and development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 39-76). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Mussen, P. H. (Ed.). (1970). Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Nesselroade, J. R., & Reese, H. W. (Eds.). (1973). Life-span developmental psychology: Methodological issues. New York: Academic Press.
Nikitin, J., & Freund, A. M. (2008). Hoping to be liked or wishing not to be rejected: Conflict and congruence of social approach and avoidance motivation. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 57, 90-111.
Novikoff, A. B. (1945a). The concept of integrative levels and biology. Science, 101, 209-215.
Novikoff, A. B. (1945b). Continuity and discontinuity in evolution. Science, 101, 405-406.
Overton, W. F. (1973). On the assumptive base of the nature-nurture controversy: Additive versus interactive conceptions. Human Development, 16, 74-89.
Overton, W. F. (1975). General systems, structure and development. In K. Riegel & G. Rosenwald (Eds.), Structure and transformation: Developmental aspects (pp. 61-81). New York: Wiley Interscience.
Overton, W. F. (1978). Klaus Riegel: Theoretical contribution to concepts of stability and change. Human Development, 21, 360-363.
Overton, W. F. (2003). Development across the life span: Philosophy, concepts, theory. In R. M. Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 6. Developmental psychology (pp. 13-42). Editor in chief: B. Weiner. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Overton, W. F. (2006). Developmental psychology: Philosophy, concepts, methodology. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.). Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development. Volume 1 of (6th ed.) (pp. 18-88). Editors-in-chief: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Overton, W., & Reese, H. (1981). Conceptual prerequisites for an understanding of stability-change and continuity-discontinuity. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 99-123.
Reese, H. W., & Overton, W. F. (1970). Models of development and theories of development. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory (pp. 115-145). New York: Academic.
Riegel, K. F. (1975) Toward a dialectical theory of human development. Human Development, 18, 50-64.
Riegel, K. F. (1976). The dialectics of human development. American Psychologist, 31, 689-700.
Riley, M. W. (Ed.). (1979). Aging from birth to death. Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science.
Sameroff, A. (1975). Transactional models in early social relations. Human Development, 18, 65-79.
Schneirla, T. C. (1957). The concept of development in comparative psychology. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development: An issue in the study of human behavior (pp. 78-108). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Stevenson, H. W. (1970). Learning in children. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 849-938). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems theories. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 563-633). Editor in chief: W. Damon. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Thompson, R. A., & Lamb, M. E. (1986). Infant-mother attachment: New directions for theory and research. In P. B. Baltes, D. L. Featherman, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 7, pp. 1-41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tobach, E., & Greenberg, G. (1984). The significance of T. C. Schneirla’s contribution to the concept of levels of integration. In G. Greenberg & E. Tobach (Eds.), Behavioral evolution and integrative levels (pp. 1-8). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. von Bertalanffy, L. (1933). Modern theories of development. London: Oxford University Press. von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General systems theory. New York: Braziller.
White, S. H. (1970). The learning theory tradition and child psychology. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 657-701). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Wohlwill, J. F. (1973). The study of behavioral development. New York: Academic Press.
Contributors
Ellen Bialystok
York University Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!

Lesen Sie weiter in der vollständigen Ausgabe!