114,99 €
The emerging understanding of age-related neurological disorders suggests that notions of a single causal gene/toxin being responsible is likely incorrect. Neurological disorders probably arise due to a unique intersection of multiple genetic and toxic factors, combined with additional contributions of age, stage of development, immune system actions, and more. This perspective leads to the view that rather than reflecting only one pathway to end-state disease, each is a spectrum disorder, and every individual case is therefore unique.
Neural Dynamics of Neurological Disease argues for a fundamental rethinking of what we think we know about neurological disorders, how they arise and progress, and, crucially, what might be done to "cure" them. It first introduces the concept of neural dynamics of neurological disease, then examines various diseases and gives examples of the interplay of elements such as neural systems, cell types, and biochemical pathways that can contribute to disease. The concluding chapters point the way to how the emerging notion of neurological disease as a dynamic process may lead to more successful treatment options.
Providing a cross-disciplinary approach to understanding the origin and progression of neurological disease, Neural Dynamics of Neurological Disease is a timely and valuable resource for neuroscientists, researchers, and clinicians.
Das E-Book können Sie in Legimi-Apps oder einer beliebigen App lesen, die das folgende Format unterstützen:
Veröffentlichungsjahr: 2017
Title Page
Copyright
Dedication
Preface
Chapter 1: Acknowledgments
Part I: The Dynamics of Neurological Disease
Chapter 1: The Dynamics of Neurological Disease: Current Views and Key Issues
1.1 Introduction
1.2 The Complexity of Human Neurological Diseases
1.3 The Nervous System as an Archetypical Complex System
1.4 CNS Signaling Failures: Implications for Neurological Disease
1.5 History and Key Characteristics of the Age-Dependent Neurological Diseases
1.6 The Fractal Nature of Complexity in the CNS
Chapter 2: Clinical and Economic Features of Age-Related Neurological Diseases
2.1 Introduction
2.2 Parkinson's Disease
2.3 Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis
2.4 Alzheimer's Disease
2.5 Summary of the Data on the Progressive, Age-Related Neurological Diseases
2.6 Neural Loci and Mechanisms of Action
Chapter 3: Spectrums of Neurological Disease, Clusters, and Ubiquity
3.1 Introduction
3.2 Spectrums of Neurological Disease
3.3 The Dimension of the Problem when Assessing Potential Causal Factors in Neurological Diseases
3.4 Neurological Disease Clusters
3.5 Ubiquity
3.6 Nested Complex Systems: Proximal versus Distal Events as They May Relate to Neurological Diseases
3.7 The Path to “Curing” Neurological Diseases
Chapter 4: Complexity, Cascading Failures, and Neurological Diseases
4.1 Introduction
4.2 Introduction to Complexity Theory and Complex Systems
4.3 Computer Programs and Computer Crashes
4.4 Biosemiosis in the CNS (Part 1)
4.5 Complexity in the CNS and the Impact of Genetic and Environmental Insults
4.6 Tipping Points and Time Lines of Disease Progression
Chapter 5: Genetic Determinants of Neurological Disease
5.1 Introduction
5.2 Causality versus Coincidence
5.3 Actions of Mutant Genes in Neurological Disease
5.4 Genetic Mutations Linked to Parkinson's Disease
5.5 Genetic Mutations Linked to ALS
5.6 Genetic Mutations Linked to Alzheimer's Disease
5.7 Genes and Neurological Disease: Some General Considerations
Chapter 6: Environmental Determinants of Neurological Disease and Gene–Toxin Interactions
6.1 Introduction
6.2 Toxins and Neurological Diseases
6.3 Aluminum and Neurological Disease
6.4 Single- vs. Multiple-Hit Models of Neurological Disease: Gene–Toxin Interactions
6.5 Genetic Susceptibility Factors
6.6 Biosemiosis (Part 2)
6.7 Gene–Toxin Interactions and Cascading Failures
6.8 Genes and Toxins in Neurological Disease: Penultimate Thoughts
6.9 And, Finally, the Microbiome
Chapter 7: The Mystery and Lessons of ALS-PDC
7.1 Introduction
7.2 Neurological Disease Clusters and ALS-PDC
7.3 History and Features of ALS-PDC
7.4 Cycad and ALS-PDC
7.5 Amino Acid Toxins in Cycad and ALS-PDC
7.6 Non-Amino Acid Toxins Linked to ALS-PDC
7.7 Aluminum and Ionic Etiologies for ALS-PDC
7.8 Still Other Molecules Causal to ALS-PDC
7.9 What is the Current View on the Importance of ALS-PDC?
7.10 Complexity of Neurological Diseases as Viewed from Guam
Part II: Age and Time Lines of Neurological Disease
Chapter 8: Neurological Disease Models and their Discontents: Validity, Replicability, and the Decline Effect
8.1 Introduction
8.2 Modeling Human Neurological Diseases: Possibilities and Pitfalls
8.3 Considerations Regarding Model Systems
8.4 Model Systems and their Discontents
8.5 Is There an Ideal Model for Studying Neurological Diseases? General Considerations
8.6 Specific Considerations for Ideal Model-System Approaches in ALS
8.7 Alternative Views of Neurological Disease and Model-Systems Approaches: Multiple-Hit Etiologies
Chapter 9: The Progression and the Time Line of Neurological Disease
9.1 Introduction
9.2 Creating Disease Time Lines: The Framingham Study
9.3 Time Lines of Neurological Disease
9.4 Back to a Multiple-Hit Disease Consideration
9.5 Haecceity and Quiddity in Context to Biosemiosis and Multiple Hits
9.6 Some Final Thoughts on Time Lines of Neurological Disease: Differentiation and Neurogenesis
Chapter 10: Development, Aging, and Neurological Disease
10.1 Introduction
10.2 The Fetal Basis of Adult Disease Hypothesis
10.3 ASD as a Developmental Neurological Disorder
10.4 Toxins and Developmental CNS Disorders
10.5 Developmental versus Mature CNS Disorders
Part III: Interactions and Synergies in Neurological Disease
Chapter 11: CNS–Immune System Interactions and Autoimmunity
11.1 Introduction
11.2 Immunity and the CNS, an Introduction to a Complex Topic
11.3 CNS–Immune System Interactions: More Detailed Considerations
11.4 Autoimmunity
11.5 Immune System Signaling Errors and Autoimmunity in ASD and Other Neurological Disorders
11.6 Laterality and Autoimmunity in Neurological Diseases
11.7 Other System Disorders in Neurological Diseases: More Evidence for Autoimmunity?
11.8 Are There Infectious Disease Links to Neurological Diseases?
Chapter 12: The Impact of Synergy of Factors in Neurological Disease
12.1 Introduction
12.2 Synergistic and Additive Effects in General and as Applied to CNS Diseases
12.3 Gene–Environment (Toxin) Interactions in Non-neuronal Systems
12.4 Gene–Environment (Toxin) Interactions in Neurological Disease
12.5 Levels of Complexity in Gene–Toxin Interactions: Implications for Current and Future Therapeutics
Part IV: Transition and Politics in Neurological Disease
Chapter 13: The Current Status of Neurological Disease Treatments
13.1 Introduction
13.2 Current Therapeutic Approaches to Treating Neurological Diseases
13.3 Summary
Chapter 14: The Future of Translational Research in Neurological Disease
14.1 Introduction
14.2 Comparing Traumatic Brain Injury to Neurological Diseases
14.3 ALS and Polio: Comparing the Nature of Neural Degeneration and Progression in the Two Diseases
14.4 Neurological Diseases as Spectrum Disorders: Implications for Therapy
14.5 Cystic Fibrosis and Gene Therapy
14.6 Restoring CNS Function: What Is the Bottom Line?
14.7 Biosemiosis (Part 3) and True Narrative Representations
Chapter 15: Defining the Limits for Neurological Disease Treatments
15.1 Introduction
15.2 The Complexity of the Human CNS versus One View of the Philosophy of Science
15.3 Examples of Unique Individuality: From Pilgrimages to Nature
15.4 Therapeutic Windows for the Treatment of Neurological Diseases
Chapter 16: The Politics and Economics of Neurological Disease
16.1 Introduction
16.2 The Problems with Single-Hit Models of Neurological Disease
16.3 Summarizing the Main Themes by Chapter
16.4 Can the Amount of Money Spent Change these Outcomes for Neurological Disease Treatment?
16.5 General Considerations for the Future of Neurological Disease Research
16.6 The Advent of Modern Dentistry and Dental Prophylaxis
16.7 Addressing Neurological Diseases at the Individual and Population Levels
Glossary
References 1: References
Index
End User License Agreement
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
Cover
Table of Contents
Preface
Begin Reading
Chapter 2: Clinical and Economic Features of Age-Related Neurological Diseases
Table 2.1 The ALSFRS scaling system for ALS
Chapter 5: Genetic Determinants of Neurological Disease
Table 5.1 Causal genes linked to Parkinson's disease
Table 5.2 Causal genes linked to ALS
Table 5.3 Causal genes linked to Alzheimer's disease
Chapter 6: Environmental Determinants of Neurological Disease and Gene–Toxin Interactions
Table 6.1 Sources of bioavailable aluminum in humans
Table 6.2 Aluminum's CNS impacts in animals and humans.
Source
: Adapted from Tomlijenovic (2011). See original article for relevant references
Table 6.3 Susceptibility genes: Parkinson's disease
Table 6.5 Susceptibility genes: Alzheimer's disease
Chapter 11: CNS–Immune System Interactions and Autoimmunity
Table 11.1 Immune system role in ASD
Chapter 13: The Current Status of Neurological Disease Treatments
Table 13.1 Recently completed Phase III randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind, parallel-group clinical trials for Parkinson's disease (PD)
Table 13.2 Recently completed Phase III randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind, parallel-group clinical trials for Alzheimer's disease (AD).
Source
: Modified from Mitsumoto et al. (2014)
Table 13.3 Drug Therapies: Alzheimer's disease. Recently completed Phase III randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind, parallel-group clinical trials for ALS
Christopher A. Shaw
Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of British Columbia, Canada
© 2017 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved
Published by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey
Published simultaneously in Canada
No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 750-4470, or on the web at www.copyright.com. Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008, or online at http://www.wiley.com/go/permission.
Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials. The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a professional where appropriate. Neither the publisher nor author shall be liable for any loss of profit or any other commercial damages, including but not limited to special, incidental, consequential, or other damages.
For general information on our other products and services or for technical support, please contact our Customer Care Department within the United States at (800) 762-2974, outside the United States at\hb (317) 572-3993 or fax (317) 572-4002.
Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats. Some content that appears in print may not be available in electronic formats. For more information about Wiley products, visit our web site at www.wiley.com.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Names: Shaw, Christopher A. (Christopher Ariel), author.
Title: Neural dynamics of neurological disease / Christopher A. Shaw.
Description: Hoboken, New Jersey : John Wiley & Sons, Inc., [2017] | Includes bibliographical references and index.\hb
Identifiers: LCCN 2016051232 (print) | LCCN 2016051536 (ebook) | ISBN 9781118634578 (cloth) | ISBN 9781118634479 (pdf) | ISBN 9781118634738 (epub)
Subjects: | MESH: Central Nervous System Diseases | Disease Progression | Aging--physiology
Classification: LCC RC346 (print) | LCC RC346 (ebook) | NLM WL 301 | DDC 616.8--dc23
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2016051232
Cover credit: © Karen Santos 2015
Cover design: Wiley
For my parents, my children, and for all peregrinos.
“Babylon in all its desolation is a sight not so awful as that of the human mind in ruins.”
Scrope Berdmore Davies1
“I can face death, but I cannot face watching myself disappear from within…I don't know who I am anymore.”
Claude Jutra2
Each annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience (SfN) is, for me, once more a reminder just how reductionist the field of neuroscience has been, continues to be, and apparently is destined to remain.
Anyone who has gone to this conference, or any similar type of large meeting, cannot help but be overwhelmed by the sheer quantity of the information on display. During the three and a half days of the main SfN meeting, some 30 000 participants will present over 15 000 posters along with almost 13 000 talks of various lengths. These numbers were the projected figures for the 2014 conference in Washington, DC, but other SfN conferences of the recent past will have been much the same in size. Future conferences will likely be even larger.
Most of the talks at the meeting occur in the so-called “mini”- and “nano”-symposia which feature 15-minute-long presentations, each usually containing a small body of data and its preliminary interpretation. However, the poster sessions really show the true dimensions of the conference: seven 4-hour-long sessions, each filling an area the size of several football fields.
Each poster, or mini-talk, contains a snippet of information – almost all of it, as noted, preliminary – a lot of which will turn out to be conceptually flawed in design or experimentally incorrect. Much of the time, as the lack of later publications bears out, the work is simply not reproducible. This outcome is in accord with studies by various scholars who have noted a lack of reproducibility in experimental data of all kinds, perhaps particularly often in the biomedical sciences.
Multiply these numbers by the additional numbers of people and presentations at conferences in neurology or more specialist neurological diseases, multiply again by the number of years these conferences have all been going on, and one likely gets billions of words and millions of tons of paper in a virtual tidal wave of information, which, combined with endless time spent by a great variety of otherwise quite talented scientists, actually produces what, at the end of the day, amounts to relatively little useful information about neurological diseases. Further, very little of this information is actually sorted, compiled, or cross-checked internally or externally with the previous decades of results from all of the similar meetings.
What then are the outcomes for neurological disease remediation? First, the field still does not understand the etiology of most neurological diseases, and, as a consequence, it has only a very limited means of translating what it thinks it knows into treatments that actually halt the progression of – never mind “cure” – these diseases.
The problem here is therefore obviously not one of quantity, or even in many cases of quality. Rather, the problem is that the field still cannot answer some really fundamental questions about the diseases in question and therefore cannot come up with treatments that make a lot of sense mechanistically or, at the very least, do what they are intended to do. Maybe what this really means is that the field of neurological disease research is not asking the right questions, or that it does not know how to interpret the answers.
For me, the question is not how to (over) simplify the nervous system and its diseases, but rather how to understand them in their entirety. Admittedly, the task of understanding the former has proven quite difficult. The second goal clearly depends on accomplishing the first.
As other authors have pointed out in different contexts, attempts to “atomitize” a subject of study into ever smaller bits without any context to their inter-relationships can be enormously detrimental (see, for example, Gould and Lewontin, 1979). Further, should the field really expect that a system as complex as the nervous system will break down in a simple way, or should it expect that its pieces will, in some measure, reflect its overall innate complexity? Almost for sure, it is the latter. At least, this is the perspective I will take in the pages that follow. I should acknowledge here that my bias against overusing reductionist approaches when considering neurological disease origins in as complex a system as the human central nervous system (CNS) is very much the polar opposite to the tack taken by Dr. Christof Koch, one of the foremost theorists on human consciousness (see Koch, 2012). The latter subject is surely as complex as the breakdown of the CNS in neurological diseases, but there may be some common ground (see Chapter 14).
As will be discussed in this book, the origin, function, and diseases of the nervous system are, by their very nature, complex, and are highly interconnected amongst the various types of cells and regions affected. The concept of biosemiosis, or biological signaling, is in this context highly relevant, and it will be highlighted in much of the discussion that follows. Moreover, the diseases upon which this book will focus are “progressive,” meaning that they continue to get worse in terms of nervous system pathology and functioning over time. They are also age-related and somewhat sex-dependent, are complicated by the added complexities of genetic variations, individual microbiomes, and a host of other likely contributing factors.
How all of these aspects combine to produce any neurological disease is actually something that neurological disease research has not really begun to understand. If the nervous system is constructed as a complex system both developmentally and functionally, which it decidedly is, then it is surely so when it malfunctions. In brief, those of us in what can broadly be described as the neurological disease “field,” a term that will be used throughout the book, are in rather dire need of a conceptual frame shift.
Many scientists are hard at work to accomplish such a shift, but they are swimming against a powerful tide of overwhelming amounts of data, which, as noted earlier, are often incorrect. How, then, is one to sort the wheat from the chaff, the valid from the invalid?
This book is intended to help the process along. Inevitably, in so doing, it will annoy some of my neuroscience colleagues as it may seem to imply that all their myriad experiments – often with amazingly spectacular methodologies – are not going to get the field to any answers without reframing the questions. Techniques are, after all, merely the equivalent of tactics in a military setting, simply, in this case, the means to accomplish the larger strategic goal of understanding these diseases. The strategic goal is aimed at an end state of prevention (or of effective treatment, as the second-best option).
Understanding this end state is actually critical to our collective wellbeing, because these various diseases are threatening to overwhelm the medical systems of the developed nations. (As for the developing countries, their medical systems are in many cases in poor enough shape as is, and hardly need the added burden of increased neurological diseases.)
My hope is that Neural Dynamics of Neurological Disease will spark debate. Time will tell if this hope has been realized. While desirable, indeed essential, from my perspective, such an outcome is decidedly a long shot. Scientific journals and meetings such as the SfN have become major industries, and are often mired in dogma, with an apparently dominant philosophy that “more equates to better.”
It is clear from the work of Prof. John Ioannidis and others that more is not necessarily better if the data are incorrect or interpreted incorrectly and/or are not verified by replication, or at least convergent forms of information. Thus, of the approximately 28 000 talks and poster presentations at SfN, some two-thirds (or more) will be incorrect, and virtually none will be replicated. This is a vastly larger problem than most of those in the field realize, and I will touch upon it further in Chapter 8.
It is reasonable to assume that much of what follows in this book will be controversial, not so much because the data are contested (although in many cases they are) but because the way I have chosen to put them together in particular categories leads to certain conclusions. Other authors, ordering the subjects in different ways, might reach very different outcomes. In this sense, the process of writing a book is a lot like museum curatorship in that what one chooses to put on display versus what one leaves in the basement will provide very different narratives. When writing about neurological diseases, how one collates and arranges the key subjects and lesser items shapes the presentation, and thus the conclusions. And, needless to say, all authors have their own assumptions, prejudices for or against certain hypotheses and data, and ways of viewing any particular field of study.
Given this, it seems only fair at the outset for me to state my own assumptions. These are listed in a sequence from what I hope will be the least controversial, “motherhood” sorts of assertions to those that perhaps deviate to a lesser or greater extent from mainstream concepts of the nervous system in disease. Each will be bolstered by the relevant literature in the appropriate places in the book's chapters.
One point to be addressed first, however, is the following: the terms “disease” and “disorder” tend to be used synonymously when speaking of those conditions that afflict the human nervous system. This consideration applies particularly to those diseases that are the main focus of this book, namely Parkinson's disease, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) (colloquially called Lou Gerhig's disease, although it might just as well have been termed Charcot's disease, as it sometimes has been), and Alzheimer's disease. Is it correct to term these conditions “diseases”? The difference between the two words can be subtle. “Disease” is normally used in the sense of sickness or illness. These neurological conditions fit this definition, and hence their names are appropriate. In addition, there is some evidence – not particularly strong, but evidence nevertheless – that they actually arise as part of an infectious process. Hence, calling them “diseases” is even more correct. In regard to the word “disorder,” various dictionaries define it to mean “an illness that disrupts normal physical or mental functions.” These conditions definitely do both, so it is equally correct to refer to them as “disorders.” Therefore, with apologies to the purists amongst the readers, the terms “neurological disease” and “neurological disorder” will be used interchangeably in the chapters that follow. When speaking of specific conditions (e.g., Alzheimer's disease), the word “disease” will always be used.
With that out of the way, I want to introduce the central theses to be addressed, not necessarily in the following order:
1.
The human CNS is complex. It contains something on the order of 86 billion neurons, organized into multiple subsystems, surrounded by 85 billion supporting glial cells. Neurons are totally dependent on these support cells for their normal functions. Each neuron connects to multiple other neurons for an estimated 94 trillion synaptic connections. There should be nothing particularly controversial about anything in this paragraph for anyone in neuroscience/neurological disease research.
2.
The complexity of the nervous system arises due to the interplay between genetic programs and environmental influences. This complexity includes the interactions that lead to neurodegeneration. Gene defects in the germ cell line and in the early developing CNS are likely to be fatal or result in profoundly disturbed neuronal functions. Environmental impacts on the CNS depend crucially on the stage of development: prenatal ones are likely to be of greater impact than those occurring in postnatal life, while early postnatal ones will be more impactful than those later in life. The concept of the “fetal basis of adult disease” used in other fields of study likely applies to neurological disease just as strongly (or even more so) to those disorders with which it is more conventionally associated. Environmental impacts also crucially depend on the number of CNS levels impacted (e.g., from genome to the whole CNS).
3.
It is almost certain that gene defects/mutations alone will not explain most types of age-related neurological disease. Nor, for that matter, will obvious environmental stressors/toxins be found to be solely responsible in most cases. Hence, gene–toxin interactions are the likely source of most such diseases, acted upon by a number of other variables across the lifespan.
4.
Neuronal compensation for genetic or environmental insults to the CNS will be limited by the type of insult and the stage(s) at which they occurs. Early gene defects, if not rapidly fatal, may be compensated for by redundancy of function of other genes. Environmental impacts, if they do not cross too many levels of organization, may allow for neuronal compensation by unaffected cells or regions. “Neuronal plasticity” is not a simple process, nor one strictly limited to the stage of neuronal development.
5.
For all of these reasons, neurological diseases that are age-related (e.g., Parkinson's disease, ALS, Alzheimer's disease, and others) are going to be complex as well. The same applies to neuronal disorders at the other end of the age spectrum (e.g., autism spectrum disorder (ASD)).
6.
At least for Parkinson's disease, ALS, Alzheimer's disease, there is only one, possibly two, real neurological clusters with a sufficient number of afflicted patients to allow effective epidemiology. The first cluster is ALS–parkinsonism dementia complex (ALS-PDC) of the Western Pacific. This includes the islands of Guam and Rota (where it was first described), Irian Jaya, and perhaps the Kii Peninsula of Japan (whether the CNS disorders in Kii are related to the others is an area of some controversy). The second possible cluster is the form of parkinsonism associated with consumption of the soursop fruit on the French Caribbean island of Guadeloupe.
7.
The gene–toxin interactions leading to neurological diseases are not CNS-specific, but impact other organ systems as well. They may not be the cause of death or nervous system dysfunction, but ignoring these other organ impacts misses a number of crucial clues to disease etiology.
8.
Still other organ systems are likely significantly involved in neurological diseases. A good example is the immune system in which autoimmune reactions may be a primary player in the onset and progression of some neurological diseases. The immune system also plays important roles in normal neuronal development.
9.
Because of the complexity and interconnectedness of the CNS, damage at any level must necessarily cascade to other levels (e.g., cell to circuit, circuit to a particular region, etc.). So-called “cascading failures” will, at some point, trigger a total system collapse. Thus, after such a critical stage is reached, no effective therapy will be possible. For this reason, therapies designed to target late stages of disease, namely most at the “clinical” diagnosis stage, will inevitably fail and may simply exacerbate rather than relieve underlying pathological processes. The concepts from biosemiosis of the “true narrative representation” (TNR) apply here.
10.
Any models of neurological diseases, no matter what kind of model or for which disease, are at best a limited means of understanding the complexity of the particular disease. They are even less effective in developing therapeutic approaches to early or late disease states.
11.
Many of the data in the literature in any of the subfields of neurological disease research are likely to be wrong and thus highly misleading. Each subfield needs a thorough review to cull such incorrect material. This is not likely to happen.
12.
Each of the sporadic/gene-susceptibility age-dependent neurological diseases represents not one entity but a spectrum of related disease states. Each case is therefore individual. Against such individual (and thus, unique) presentations, there can never be a generalized treatment. This applies particularly if treatment options are begun post-diagnosis. Effective treatments for neurological diseases, if they occur at all, can arise only from prophylaxis or the next-best option of extremely early-phase detection followed by strategic, targeted therapy. The only way to get to this stage is for governments and other entities to commit significant funds to providing a new perspective on such diseases. Essentially, this is a policy discussion, in which social priorities need to be carefully examined. Policy considerations are not the traditional role of scientists, but without the input of those doing the research, a policy re-evaluation will almost certainly not happen. Whether it does or does not is a choice. Needless to say, choices have consequences.
These last comments are really the focus of this book, and were fleshed out from some very preliminary thoughts as I walked the Camino Frances of the Camino de Santiago. For those who do not know it, the Camino actually describes a number of routes, mostly in Spain and France, which all end up in the Galician city of Santiago de Compostella. Even on a single route, although the conventional end point remains the same, the geography can vary from year to year, as a result of human activity and weather. How one actually walks the Camino varies with season, personal fitness, past or acquired injuries, frame of mind, companions, and so on. Not everyone finishes. For those who do complete the Camino, no two journeys are the same. Thus, one often hears the expression, “walking one's own Camino.”
All of this leads to the point hinted at earlier: no two neurological disease manifestations, even in ostensibly the same disease, are actually the same, except perhaps at disease end state. Everyone walks their own Camino of neurological health. This metaphor, I think, has significant implications for neurological disease detection and treatment.
Four final points – caveats, really – need to be acknowledged, all of which will be discernible to readers in due course. First, just as neurological diseases are not linear in how they develop, progress, or complete, this book is not linear either. While there is a trajectory that leads from the first pages to the final conclusions, the book could not be written as if it were a simple story. Rather, it is recursive in fact and concept, with various themes being introduced and then reconsidered pages later as new information is added. Some readers may find that this makes parts of the book redundant. I hope, however, that such readers will see that any one such theme is expanded by the stage of the book and the discussions that have occurred since it was last raised.
Second, in some sections I describe the work of my laboratory and colleagues in more detail than I do the work of others. The reason is simple: I know my own work best – the valid parts as well as the invalid. I hope I have not done such self-selection too blatantly, or too often.
Third, in areas that are likely to prove particularly controversial, I err on the side of providing too many, rather than too few, primary literature references. This point ties in with the fourth caveat: The book is written mostly for my fellow neuroscientists and for those in the neurological disease world. This focus inevitably leads to some pretty dense – and reference-filled – expositions, which may be daunting for any nonspecialist scientists or the lay public. A glossary is provided at the end, which I hope will help.
That about sums it up.
Needless to say, in all of the following material, any errors in citation, content, or interpretation are purely my own.
Christopher A. Shaw
Victoria, BC, Canada
1
Scope Berdmore Davies (1782–1852) was a dandy and friend of Lord Byron.
2
Claude Jutra (1930–1986) was a Quebecois director, screenwriter, film editor, cinematographer, and actor. After being diagnosed with Alzheimer's and living with the condition for a time, Jutra committed suicide. In recent years, his reputation has been stained by allegations of pedophilia.
This book owes much to a number of individuals, for a great variety of reasons. First, I thank Justin Jeffryes of John Wiley & Sons for suggesting the project and for tolerating my numerous requests for extensions. The various support persons at Wiley were a pleasure to work with throughout the entire process and I am most grateful for their efforts on behalf of the book. Next, thanks are due to Claire Dwoskin for her financial support to my laboratory and her boundless encouragement in all things. Other supporters were the Luther Allyn Dean Shourds estate, the Kaitlin Fox Foundation, and various more official granting agencies, including the National Institutes of Health and the US Department of Defense.
My laboratory mates provided endless enthusiasm and cogent conversation, acted as sounding boards, and offered much other help as the project emerged. In no particular order, these were: Dr. Alice Li, Sneha Sheth, Jess Morrice, and others listed more specifically in the following.
Janice Yoo and Jessie Holbeck were most helpful in tracking down references and summarizing blocks of data for various chapters. In regard to the data summaries, Janice in particular was an amazing source, and her summaries enabled various sections to be completed. Indeed, without Janice, the book would not have been finished in anything like a timely manner. As the final deadline loomed, Janice took over all of the formatting and reference checking. For all of these reasons, my gratitude to her is boundless. Katie Blank did a fantastic job with the figures and tolerated my endless revisions. I also owe her a special debt of gratitude. Michael Kuo did final formatting, reference checks, and a huge range of jobs associated with getting this book through the copy editing and galley proof stages. I owe him a huge debt of gratitude as well.