The Sociology of Boredom - Mariusz Finkielsztein - E-Book

The Sociology of Boredom E-Book

Mariusz Finkielsztein

0,0
36,99 €

-100%
Sammeln Sie Punkte in unserem Gutscheinprogramm und kaufen Sie E-Books und Hörbücher mit bis zu 100% Rabatt.
Mehr erfahren.
Beschreibung

A pioneering sociological exploration of boredom as a culturally- and socially-dependent emotion

Does boredom have a history?
What can hunter-gatherers teach us about boredom?
Is boredom experienced differently by those in different socio-economic classes?
Is boredom a disease that is now globalized in a world of inequalities and marginalization?
Does boredom contribute to political movements, wars, terrorism, or cultural revolutions?
What does boredom have to do with power?
How do high expectations contribute to being recurrently bored?

In The Sociology of Boredom, Mariusz Finkielsztein provides a new approach to conceptualizing, interpreting, and perceiving one of the most widespread, yet neglected, human emotions. Investigating boredom at both the macro- and micro-sociological level, Finkielsztein develops an original relational-expectational theory as he explores boredom through the lenses of different social structure theories, interactionist theory, historical sociology, sociology of emotions, essentialism and constructivism, and social anthropology.

Eight in-depth chapters examine the social production of boredom in modernity and late modernity, addressing topics such as the boredom of marginalized groups, the concepts of busy boredom and consumer boredom, the characteristics and consequences of workplace boredom, the notion of smart boredom in the information society, and more.

Containing detailed analyses of the nature of boredom and its connections to various spheres of social life, The Sociology of Boredom is essential reading for advanced undergraduates, postgraduates, university lecturers, and academic researchers in sociology and similar disciplines, particularly those involved in studies on emotions or boredom studies.

Sie lesen das E-Book in den Legimi-Apps auf:

Android
iOS
von Legimi
zertifizierten E-Readern

Seitenzahl: 863

Veröffentlichungsjahr: 2025

Bewertungen
0,0
0
0
0
0
0
Mehr Informationen
Mehr Informationen
Legimi prüft nicht, ob Rezensionen von Nutzern stammen, die den betreffenden Titel tatsächlich gekauft oder gelesen/gehört haben. Wir entfernen aber gefälschte Rezensionen.



Table of Contents

Cover

Table of Contents

Title Page

Copyright Page

Dedication Page

Illustrations

About the Author

Acknowledgements

References

Introduction: Upstream Psychological Trend

In Search of the Sociology of Boredom

Boredom as a Social Emotion

Defining Boredom

The Structure of the Book

References

1 Boredom and Modernity

Does Boredom Have a History?

Boredom as a Social Construct of Modernity

References

2 Late Modern Boredom

Busy Boredom

Social Placebos and Smart Boredom

Overload: Redundancy, Noise, and Inattention

Consumer Boredom

Artificial Needs

The Addiction to Stimuli, Novelty, and Pleasure

Change for Change's Sake: Fashion

The Boredom Industry and the Failure of Re‐enchantment

Boredom as a Fundamental Mood of Consumerism

References

3 Boredom and Social Inequality

Boredom as a Class Issue

Boredom of the Marginalized

Boredom and the Capitalistic World‐system

References

4 Workplace Boredom

Work as a Boring Concept

Causes of Workplace Boredom

Outcomes of Workplace Boredom

References

5 Religious Boredom

Boredom in the Sphere of Religion

Acedia

Church Boredom

References

6 Boredom and Social Change

Boredom and Social (De)Mobilization

Boredom and Violence

References

7 Boredom and Utopia

Literary Utopias

“Real” Utopias

References

8 Interactional Boredom

Interactionist Approach

Boredom and Power

Boredom on Display

Romantic Relationship Boredom

References

Conclusion: The Relational‐expectationalTheory of Boredom

Premise 1: Boredom Is a Socially‐constructed Emotion

Premise 2: Boredom Is a Relational State

Premise 3: Boredom Is Caused by a Failure of Expectations

Premise 4: Expectation‐Driven Boredom Is a Civilizational Trait

Premise 5: Expectations (in the Form of Desires/Dreams/Aspirations) Make the Reality Boring

Premise 6: Expectation‐driven Boredom Is a Function of Social Comparisons

Premise 7: Expectations May Lead to Anticipatory Boredom

Remedies

References

Further Reading

Index

End User License Agreement

List of Tables

Chapter 1

Table 1.1: Types of essentialism and constructivism

Table 1.2: Types of suicide identified by Durkheim

Table 1.3: The influence of unbalanced integration and regulation on boredo...

Table 1.4: Typology of boredom (integration vs regulation) based on Durkhei...

Chapter 2

Table 2.1: Sectors of information search

Chapter 4

Table 4.1: Workload typology

Table 4.2: Types of approach to work and the level of work‐related boredom...

Table 4.3: Types of empty labour

Conclusion

Table 9.1 Causes and remedies for boredom

List of Illustrations

Chapter 2

Figure 2.1 The spiral of consumer boredom.

Chapter 5

Figure 5.1 The relationship between acedia, boredom, laziness, sloth, and me...

Guide

Cover Page

Table of Contents

Title Page

Copyright Page

Dedication Page

Illustrations

About the Author

Acknowledgements

Begin Reading

Conclusion: The Relational‐expectational Theory of Boredom

Further Reading

Index

Wiley End User License Agreement

Pages

iii

iv

v

xi

xii

xiii

xiv

xv

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

33

34

35

36

37

38

39

40

41

42

43

44

45

46

47

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

57

58

59

60

61

62

63

64

65

66

67

68

69

70

71

72

73

74

75

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

83

84

85

86

87

88

89

90

91

92

93

94

95

96

97

98

99

100

101

102

103

104

105

106

107

108

109

110

111

112

113

114

115

116

117

118

119

120

121

122

123

124

125

126

127

128

129

130

131

132

133

134

135

136

137

138

139

140

141

142

143

144

145

146

147

148

149

150

151

152

153

154

155

156

157

158

159

160

161

162

163

164

165

166

167

168

169

170

171

172

173

174

175

176

177

178

179

180

181

182

183

184

185

186

187

188

189

190

191

192

193

194

195

196

197

198

199

200

201

202

203

204

205

206

207

208

209

210

211

212

213

214

215

216

217

218

219

220

221

222

223

224

225

226

227

228

229

230

231

232

233

234

235

236

237

238

239

240

241

242

243

244

245

246

247

248

249

250

251

252

253

254

255

256

257

258

259

260

261

262

263

264

265

266

267

268

269

270

271

272

273

274

275

276

277

278

279

280

281

282

283

284

285

286

287

288

289

290

291

292

293

294

295

296

297

298

299

300

301

302

303

304

305

306

307

308

309

310

311

312

313

314

315

316

317

318

319

320

321

322

323

324

325

326

327

328

329

330

331

332

The Sociology of Boredom

Mariusz Finkielsztein

This edition first published 2025© 2025 John Wiley & Sons Ltd

All rights reserved, including rights for text and data mining and training of artificial intelligence technologies or similar technologies. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, except as permitted by law. Advice on how to obtain permission to reuse material from this title is available at http://www.wiley.com/go/permissions.

The right of Mariusz Finkielsztein to be identified as the author of this work has been asserted in accordance with law.

Registered OfficesJohn Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, USAJohn Wiley & Sons Ltd, New Era House, 8 Oldlands Way, Bognor Regis, West Sussex, PO22 9NQ, UK

For details of our global editorial offices, customer services, and more information about Wiley products visit us at www.wiley.com.

The manufacturer’s authorized representative according to the EU General Product Safety Regulation is Wiley‐VCH GmbH, Boschstr. 12, 69469 Weinheim, Germany, e‐mail: [email protected].

Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats and by print‐on‐demand. Some content that appears in standard print versions of this book may not be available in other formats.

Trademarks: Wiley and the Wiley logo are trademarks or registered trademarks of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and/or its affiliates in the United States and other countries and may not be used without written permission. All other trademarks are the property of their respective owners. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. is not associated with any product or vendor mentioned in this book.

Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of WarrantyWhile the publisher and authors have used their best efforts in preparing this work, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this work and specifically disclaim all warranties, including without limitation any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives, written sales materials or promotional statements for this work. This work is sold with the understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering professional services. The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a specialist where appropriate. The fact that an organization, website, or product is referred to in this work as a citation and/or potential source of further information does not mean that the publisher and authors endorse the information or services the organization, website, or product may provide or recommendations it may make. Further, readers should be aware that websites listed in this work may have changed or disappeared between when this work was written and when it is read. Neither the publisher nor authors shall be liable for any loss of profit or any other commercial damages, including but not limited to special, incidental, consequential, or other damages.

Library of Congress Cataloging‐in‐Publication Data Applied for:

Paperback ISBN: 9781394253333

Cover Design: WileyCover Image: © Annie Runkel, 2020

To my wife

la mia volpe, la mia topolina, la mia più saggia strega

‘a ma vurpi, ‘u ma topu, ‘a ma mavara cchiu saggia

Illustrations

Figures

2.1

The spiral of consumer boredom

5.1

The relationship between acedia, boredom, laziness, sloth, and melancholy

Tables

1.1

Types of essentialism and constructivism

1.2

Types of suicide identified by Durkheim

1.3

The influence of unbalanced integration and regulation on boredom

1.4

Typology of boredom (integration vs regulation) based on Durkheim

2.1

Sectors of information search

4.1

Workload typology

4.2

Types of approach to work and the level of work‐related boredom

4.3

Types of empty labour1569.1Causes and remedies for boredom

About the Author

Mariusz Finkielsztein is an Assistant Professor at the Institute of Sociology, University of Gdańsk, Poland. He is a boredom researcher, the Secretary of the International Society of Boredom Studies, editor‐in‐chief of the Journal of Boredom Studies, and founder and organizer of the International Interdisciplinary Boredom Conference. His previous book is Boredom and Academic Work (Routledge, 2021). He obtained his PhD from the University of Warsaw. A qualitative sociologist, he is interested in boredom, the sociology of emotions, work, higher education, and creative occupations (ballroom dancers).

Website: https://mariuszfinkielsztein.com/

Acknowledgements

This book is the outcome of 10 years of reflection on boredom. It was a long and challenging intellectual process and quite an interesting journey. Although I would like to think of myself as a self‐made man, this book would probably never have come into existence without the support and/or inspiration from at least several people. I would like to express my deepest gratitude to the following:

Izabela Wagner, my mentor and advisor during my whole sociological studies, for her open‐mindedness, unconditional support, unbreakable belief in me and my “quirky” subject. She was supportive of this project from its very beginning, always encouraging me not to be afraid to aim high.

Michael Gardiner for a valuable comments on

Chapter 7

, “Boredom and Utopia” and important literature suggestions.

John Eastwood and James Danckert, whose book,

Out of My Skull: The Psychology of Boredom

, was my initial inspiration to write

The Sociology of Boredom

(Danckert & Eastwood,

2020

). Their book was the direct impetus that pushed me into working on this project. Indirect motivation came from Professor Guido Borelli, who advised me back in 2016 in Venice to write a manual on boredom that would steadily connect my name with boredom studies—this book partially consummates his counsel.

Josefa Ros Velasco, the President of the International Society of Boredom Studies, for her organizational work on establishing boredom studies and inspiration for further development of that field as well as for co‐organizing with me the fourth and fifth editions of the International Interdisciplinary Boredom Conference (June 2021 and June 2023). Inspiration and encouragement have also come from all the participants of the Boredom Conferences over these years (since 2015) in the form of fruitful discussions and interesting presentations.

Some parts of the current book have previously appeared or been published in other works of mine. Here is the list of such “borrowings” (they are all reproduced here with the permission of the publishers).

Some parts of

Chapters 1

,

2

, and

4

constitute extended and amended fragments of the unpublished PhD thesis, “On the Social Significance of Boredom: The Phenomenon of Boredom in the University Milieu” (Finkielsztein,

2019

, pp. 101–116, 149–162).

The section “Consumer Boredom” in

Chapter 2

has previously been published as an article in the

European Journal of American Studies

under the title, “Consumer Boredom: Boredom as a Subliminal Mood of Consumer Capitalism” (Finkielsztein,

2022

).

The major part of the section “Boredom in Refugee Camps: Anomic Boredom” in

Chapter 3

and the section “Strategic Boredom: Boredom and Social Control” in

Chapter 8

, constitute edited fragments of the article, “Strategic Boredom: The Experience and Dynamics of Boredom in Refugee Camp. A Mediterranean Case” in the

Journal of Contemporary Ethnography

(Wagner & Finkielsztein,

2021

, pp. 5–6, 19, 23–25).

The extract in the section “Precarious Employment: Lack of Belonging” in

Chapter 4

is derived from the chapter, “From Passionate Engagement to Chronic Boredom in Polish Academia: An Overview of Early‐Career Motivation and Systemic Contributory Factors” in the book,

Early Career Teachers

(Finkielsztein,

2021b

, pp. 126–127).

The first part of

Chapter 7

constitutes a re‐edited and extended version of the chapter, “Boredom and Melancholy in Utopias and Dystopias” in the book,

More after More

(Finkielsztein,

2016

, pp. 104–114).

The sections “Role Distance” and “Low Emotional Energy” in

Chapter 8

were published originally in the book,

Boredom and Academic Work

(Finkielsztein,

2021a

, pp. 63–66).

The section “Interactional Ritual and Anticipatory Boredom” in

Chapter 8

is based on the unpublished MA thesis, “Boredom during University Classes: The Perception of Boredom among Students of the University of Warsaw” (Finkielsztein,

2013

, pp. 68–71) and short fragments of the article, “Class‐Related Boredom among University Students: A Qualitative Research on Boredom Coping Strategies” in the

Journal of Further Education

(Finkielsztein,

2020

, pp. 1107, 1110).

The section “Boredom and Power” in

Chapter 8

is based partially on the article, “Boredom and Power: How Power Relations Influence Feeling Boredom” published in the journal

Zoon Politikon

(Finkielsztein,

2023

, pp. 127–134).

References

Danckert, J., & Eastwood, J. (2020).

Out of my skull: The psychology of boredom

. Harvard University Press.

Finkielsztein, M. (2013).

Nuda na zajęciach uniwersyteckich

.

Percepcja nudy wśród studentów Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego [Boredom during university classes. the perception of boredom among students of the University of Warsaw]

. [MA thesis, the University of Warsaw].

Finkielsztein, M. (2016). Boredom and melancholy in utopias and dystopias. In K. Olkusz, M. Kłosiński, & K. Maj (Eds.),

More after More: Essays commemorating the five hundredth anniversary of Thomas More's utopia

(pp. 104–117). Facta Ficta Research Centre.

Finkielsztein, M. (2019).

On the social significance of boredom: The phenomenon of boredom in the university milieu

. [PhD thesis, the University of Warsaw].

Finkielsztein, M. (2020). Class‐related academic boredom among university students: A qualitative research on boredom coping strategies.

Journal of Further and Higher Education

,

44

(8), 1098–1113.

https://doi.org/10.1080/0309877X.2019.1658729

Finkielsztein, M. (2021a).

Boredom and academic work

. Routledge.

Finkielsztein, M. (2021b). From passionate engagement to chronic boredom in Polish academia: An overview of early‐career motivation and systemic contributory factors. In J. Crutchley, Z. Nahaboo, & N. Rao (Eds.),

Early career teachers: International narratives of transitions within higher education

(pp. 123–134). Bloomsbury.

Finkielsztein, M. (2022). Consumer boredom: Boredom as a subliminal mood of consumer capitalism.

European Journal of American Studies

,

17

(4).

https://journals.openedition.org/ejas/18977

Finkielsztein, M. (2023). Boredom and power. How power relations influence feeling boredom.

Zoon Politikon

,

14

, 123–147.

https://doi.org/10.4467/2543408XZOP.23.004.18504

Wagner, I., & Finkielsztein, M. (2021). Strategic boredom: The experience and dynamics of boredom in refugee camp. A Mediterranean case.

Journal of Contemporary Ethnography

,

50

(5), 649–682.

https://doi.org/10.1177/08912416211008525

Introduction: Upstream Psychological Trend

In Search of the Sociology of Boredom

When I started to research boredom 14  years ago, I dreamt about a sociology of boredom being established. Over these years, boredom studies have been developing at a fair pace and more and more research covering the subject has been published every year. Still, though, psychology seems to dominate the field. In my previous book, Boredom and Academic Work (Finkielsztein, 2021), I showed that out of 572 scientific publications on boredom that I analysed, almost half (273) were from the field of psychology, and this estimate would be higher if we included in that category publications from the field of education that are based almost entirely on psychological theories and methodology. I found only 35 publications that can be classified as “sociological” and fewer than 20 that were written by sociologists. Boredom being still mostly perceived as a strictly individual affliction is the most probable reason for the neglect of the social dimension of the feeling (Anderson, 2004; Darden & Marks, 1999; Kenny, 2009; Misztal, 2016; Zijderveld, 1979). From my perspective, there is still no sociology of boredom, and the conclusion of Donna Darden and Alan Marks (1999) that “[s]ociology has largely ignored boredom, although producing a rather large amount of it” (p. 33) seems still to be an adequate description of the state of the art. Even the sociology of emotions neglects boredom (the only exception I'm aware of is Jacobsen, 2019). Sociologists of previous generations usually never mentioned boredom (the rare exceptions are evaluated in this book), and sociology has simply not dealt with boredom and, therefore, has no tradition in that regard. There are no counterparts in sociology for the classics of psychodynamic approach by Otto Fenichel (1951), Ralph Greenson (1951, 1953), or Victor Frankl (2000), producing weighty texts on boredom. Similarly, sociology has not had its Kierkegaard, Schopenhauer, or Heidegger who have devoted in‐depth treatises to the issue of boredom. There are mostly crumbs and scattered traces of indirect mentions of boredom with only a few exceptions (such as Orin Klapp's [1986] book, Overload and Boredom). I had a moment of great excitement only once when I saw the journal article by Michael Gardiner (2012), “Henri Lefebvre and the ‘Sociology of Boredom’”.1 But it was just a false alarm, a false hope—there was no real sociology of boredom included. The article “only” reconstructed the thought of one thinker and did not advocate for a sociological subdiscipline of the sociology of boredom. Admittedly, Henri Lefebvre (2002) stated that “[o]n the horizon of the modern world dawns the black sun of boredom, and the critique of everyday life has a sociology of boredom as part of its agenda” (p. 75)—yet, he seemed never to take that part of its agenda more seriously. Boredom has continued to be a rather minor mention of no serious consideration in sociology, and was frequently used in a taken‐for‐granted and colloquial manner (Darden & Marks, 1999). Given that situation, I decided to take the sociology of boredom into my own hands.

The recent publication of Out of My Skull: The Psychology of Boredom by James Danckert and John Eastwood (2020), not to mention the already classic Philosophy of Boredom by Lars Svendsen (2005), made it even more apparent that in the field of boredom studies there is still a lack of serious sociological voices. It is even more pressing in the face of boredom being a well‐established subject of other disciplines' investigations (measured by the number of papers published every year). The perception of boredom as a strictly individual phenomenon seems to constitute the prime reason for the lack of a sociological study of boredom and a more in‐depth sociological analysis of boredom comparable to Danckert and Eastwood's in psychology or Svendsen's in philosophy. This book aims to fill this gap by providing a summative and theoretical reflection upon boredom as a social phenomenon. The Sociology of Boredom is meant to do the following:

Collect and present all the scattered strands of social/sociological reflection and research on boredom in one place.

Contribute to the introduction of new ways of conceptualizing, interpreting, and perceiving boredom, that is, to develop the further sociological analysis of boredom as a socially‐constructed emotion, by using existing sociological theories.

Propose a new theory of boredom that highlights social and interactional aspects of the phenomenon, which I would call the relational‐expectational theory of boredom.

Although the title of this book is The Sociology of Boredom, it is not meant only for sociologists. In some parts of the book, I interpret boredom in terms of classic sociological theories and provide some basic information about them—yet, this book is not a manual, but aims at demonstrating the significance and usefulness of sociology, and the sociological view in general, for an analysis and understanding of boredom. This message is directed to sociologists and non‐sociologists alike—to the former, for encouragement and inspiration for further studies, to the latter, as a supplement to previous and/or discovery of novel perspectives on boredom.

The book provides a summary of previous studies and theoretical developments and a reinterpretation of boredom in light of the sociological perspective. It is based on my reflections accumulated over 10 years of researching and theorizing on boredom and uses many of my previous ideas scattered throughout various venues (papers, chapters, theses) but constitutes an entirely novel project, as all preceding work that is incorporated into this book has been rethought and rearranged to produce a novel series of inter‐connected theoretical essays. Each chapter analyses boredom in different spheres or levels of social life, together creating a wide horizontal picture of boredom in modernity and late modernity, showing the mechanisms of the social production of boredom. At times, I employ the perspectives of social anthropology, which provides a lot of valuable material on the social dimension of boredom, and of philosophy, which offers a theoretical framework for discussing various sociological issues. In that sense, therefore, this book is not purely sociological, as I cannot avoid a pinch of heretical multi‐disciplinarity. The book will present boredom from the various sociological vantage points of the sociology of work, the sociology of social change and theories of social mobilization, the sociology of religion, theories of social structure, interactionist theory, historical sociology, the sociology of emotions, and social anthropology. It will start with macro‐sociological perspectives covering various spheres of social life and continue down to micro‐sociological analysis (see the overview of the structure of the book later in this chapter).

The book may seem to demonstrate a black‐and‐white picture of boredom at times, but this is the result of the multiplication of approaches, a polyphony of voices that mingle within each chapter. There are probably even parts of the book that present apparently oppositional perspectives on boredom. All of this is caused by shifting the perspectives and the fact that sometimes I merely refer to what has been researched and written on the matter, and at other times I develop my own reflections. The second important note is that consecutive chapters show only some aspects of the phenomenon, and even if they discuss more general theoretical issues (such as work), I tried to keep them as focused on boredom as possible. As a consequence, some statements may sound like exaggerations but actually this is just a matter of narrowing the attention to only one emotion. The landscape of my reflection is always limited. Dealing with boredom academically forces one to be an expert in many subjects beyond mere experience of that emotion. The bitter truth is yet that one cannot be an expert in everything, and I would not pretend that I am an expert on every single subject that I have raised in this book because I definitely am not. My expertise lies in boredom, I am a boredom researcher and boredom expert. This is why experts in the given fields would probably add tons of footnotes, reservations, and comments to each chapter. I am aware of that, but the aim of this book is different. It is not meant to be the last word on the subject of the sociology of boredom but, conversely, the invitation to further work to establish the field. This is not the end of the road but its beginning. I ask readers to bear that in mind while reading and finding exceptions and limitations to my statements.

Boredom as a Social Emotion

The starting point of this work is the premise that emotions are social phenomena—they are not merely an innate physiological processes or individual psychological states, but are also socially constructed. Social constructivists emphasize that “cultural ideologies, beliefs, and norms as they impinge on social structures define what emotions are to be experienced and how these culturally defined emotions are to be expressed” (Turner & Stets, 2009, p. 2). Social processes have a significant impact on emotions and, as shown by sociology of emotions, “considerable variability exists historically and cross‐culturally in the situational causes, experience, meaning, display, and regulation of emotions” (Thoits, 1989, p. 319). Social construction theorists (Gordon, Averill), and symbolic interactionists (Hochschild, Shott) view emotions as primarily dependent on definitions of the situation, emotion vocabularies, and emotional beliefs, which vary across time and location. Such “variability between and within societies can be seen as a reliable indicator that subjective emotions are socially acquired and structured” (Ohlmeier et al., 2020, p. 3). In this way, if boredom is an emotion, it is socially constructed as well.

In popular discourse, boredom is frequently equated with lack of emotions, being described as “a state of emotional deficiency”, or “affective lack” (Ngai, 2005, pp. 268, 269), but in contrast to that vision, it actually constitutes a fully‐fledged emotion. Boredom (at least situational boredom) perfectly fits into existing definitions of emotions—it is a short‐lived, subjective, psycho‐physiological affective state that has five components (Macklem, 2015, pp. 8–9; Mulligan & Scherer, 2012, p. 352): (a) affective (an unpleasant, negative feeling), (b) physiological (a non‐optimal level of arousal), (c) cognitive (a low level of attention, the perception of time passing slowly), (d) motivational (disinclination towards the activity/situation at hand and urge to change it), and (e) expressive (a slumped posture, drowsiness). More to the point here, according to Thoits' conceptualization (1989, p. 318) emotions in general involve: (a) appraisals of a situational stimulus or context; (b) changes in physiological or bodily sensations; (c) the free or inhibited display of expressive gestures; and (d) a cultural label applied to specific constellations of one or more of the first three components. First of all, “an emotion is a reaction to a situation, usually of social origin, such as a change in a social relationship. The person responds to the situation as he/she interprets it, so this is both a cognitive and social component” (Gordon, 1990, p. 152). Second, a physiological component from the biological point of view can be something objective, but only by learning culturally transmitted meanings of these sensations can people achieve a particular interpretation of them (see the case of marihuana users in Becker, 1966). Third, expressions of emotions are not universal but socially‐learned (even in the case of primary emotions). And, lastly, many theorists observed that bodily sensations in the case of emotions are rather unspecified and ambiguous, it is culture that creates distinctions between very similar physiological reactions and similar states are labelled differently in various cultures. The sociological definition of emotions “shifts the focus to the societal interpretations, patterns and rules which influence the experience and appraisal of boredom” (Ohlmeier et al., 2020, p. 4). Boredom fulfils all the criteria for being a social emotion included in the above conceptualization.

The main aim of this book is to present boredom as a socially‐constructed phenomenon and to prove that boredom can be perceived in the same way to suicide, which was viewed as having a strictly individual/psychological nature, and which was famously shown by Durkheim (2005) to be a socially dependent phenomenon as well. People produce boredom in their relations with the social environment (on various levels from direct interaction with people to their relations with institutions, social norms, etc.). Boredom is also culturally‐ and historically‐dependent—it is not merely a psychological state, nor is it uniform, regardless of culture; it is a socially‐constructed secondary emotion that can be felt, perceived, and/or displayed differently in various cultures and/or social classes. In other words, boredom is a socially‐produced emotion, it is relational and social in nature, similar to other emotions.

Defining Boredom

Taking into account in how many spheres of late modern life boredom is present, or even plays a significant role, I would claim that we all now live in the society of boredom (analogous to Ulrich Beck's risk society). The COVID‐19 pandemic has made us realize with powerful force that boredom is a society‐wide issue dependent on social factors—for instance, lockdowns and restrictions on social interactions that turned out to be circumstances conducive to boredom growing and spreading (see, among others, Ohlmeier et al., 2022; Westgate et al., 2022). Yet, the paradox of boredom lies in the fact that while being quite a serious social phenomenon, it is ignored and disregarded on a massive scale. Boredom is like the air that surrounds us and “has now reached a state of such intimate familiarity that we often fail to recognize that we are bored” (Lefebvre, 1984, p. 186; see also Svendsen, 2005, p. 14). Boredom, like other emotions, may well go unnoticed or “function below the threshold of awareness” (Scheff, 1988, pp. 397–398, in Barbalet, 2004, p. 80) and the majority of people (scientists and scholars included) “rarely wonder about boredom” (Lombardo, 2017, p. 37) and rarely take “it to be a serious phenomenon for investigation” (Gamsby, 2012, p. 350). Still, many people resent the mere idea of being bored and vigorously deny the possibility of ever feeling bored. This situation, I suppose, is basically a function of a simplistic conceptualization of boredom.

In the course of various discussions on boredom on the internet or among friends as a boredom researcher, I have noticed that people often misunderstand this phenomenon and divide themselves into those who identify boredom with idleness and those for whom it is synonymous with rest and relaxation. When asked about boredom, the first group responds: “I am never bored. I do not know what it is like to be bored. I do not understand people who get bored”, because, as we allegedly all know: “an intelligent person is never bored”. A negative attitude towards boredom and a sense of superiority are clearly visible in these statements (cf. boredom superiority effect in Gallegos et al., 2022)—how one can waste their time on boredom‐idleness clearly surpasses human comprehension and is equated with the defect of laziness. Representatives of the second group speak about boredom in a nostalgic and dreamy tone, generally in the context of having no time for it. Such people generally “dream about boredom”, while others “shamelessly” declare: “I was bored a good chunk of last weekend, and it was wonderful.” Here, boredom is equated with relaxation and evaluated positively. Such people often endorse the recently fashionable slogan that “boredom is creative”. In this sense, boredom has also become part of the campaign for slow life—“slow down, make time for boredom in your life”. In general, in our current achievement society (Han, 2015), boredom is regarded either as contemptible idleness or desired but not easily acquired rest. In extreme cases it may even be regarded as a mild form of resistance against the cultural imperative to be in a constant hurry and always meticulously occupied.

However, based on my extensive theoretical research into the matter of conceptualizing boredom, which took almost half of my previous book, Boredom and Academic Work (Finkielsztein, 2021), it can be concluded that boredom is definitely not synonymous with either rest or idleness. Boredom, according to almost all its definitions, is a negative, or rather negatively perceived, emotion. Its possible effects may be positive (creativity) or negative (aggression), it can serve a vital function, but as an affective experience, boredom is unpleasant and should be so, because it is an important signal that something should be changed, that the situation we find ourselves in is uninteresting, unstimulating, and/or meaningless for us (Elpidorou, 2017). Boredom is therefore not the same as relaxation, the essence of which is that it is judged to be something desirable. As for idleness, on the other hand, early psychological theories admittedly associated boredom with a lack of stimulation (or even sensory deprivation), and also a lack of activity, but boredom is not idleness, because then its opposite, and also its remedy, would be work, and as many people—not to mention a number of studies—can attest, boredom at work is endemic. Idleness, therefore, would be just one possible cause of boredom or of the circumstances surrounding its experience.

Boredom is also sometimes equated with apathy. Yet it is exactly a life without boredom that would be filled with apathy and stagnation. “The apathetic person is free of the pressured desire to find engagement: By its very definition, apathy is an absence of any desire to even bother redressing a lack of challenge—a failure of motivation, classically embodied in the couch potato” (Danckert & Eastwood, 2020, p. 54). Boredom, on the other hand, is described as a “desire for desires” (Tolstoy, 2002) or “the will to will” (Fernandez, 2006) “The bored person is tormented by a wanting without knowing the conditions for satisfying that want” (Danckert & Eastwood, 2020, p. 29). Boredom is, therefore, a state that includes a strong component of restlessness. More to the point, I claim that boredom is a combination of lethargy and restlessness (cf. Danckert et al., 2018), a mixture of oppositely directed listlessness and restlessness. “[T]o be bored is to experience listlessness and restlessness at the same time, from which the former is directed towards the situation at hand and the latter is focused on a prospective activity, thus, on escaping from the anaesthetizing experience” (Finkielsztein, 2021, p. 74).

Many researchers and theorists have claimed repetition and monotony to be an essential quality of boredom (Brodsky, 1995; Davies, 1926; Drory, 1982; Hill, 1975; O'Hanlon, 1981). Yet, simplifying boredom to that dimension is inaccurate, as a number of studies have suggested otherwise. First of all, in industrial work, the epitome of monotony and repetitiveness, many workers express even a preference for routine tasks as those enable them to split their attention and focus their thoughts elsewhere (Watt, 2002). Second, when the skillset is expanding, the capacity to sustain, or even enjoy, repetition increases and boredom remains out of the picture. In this connection, Richard Sennett (2008) discusses the so‐called Isaac Stern rule in music, stating that “the better your technique, the longer you can rehearse without becoming bored” (p. 38, in Danaher, 2019, p. 241). This rule is corroborated by Izabela Wagner (2015), who observed that classical musicians (violins and pianists) seem never to be bored with the routine exercises (except for some child musicians). From my own experience as a ballroom dancer, I can draw similar conclusions. I was never bored with repeating limited sets of basic moves and steps when I was practising them and I realized it only by contrast to teaching others. In general, therefore, if a routine is meaningful and purposeful, it does not prompt boredom and may even provide actors with a sense of security and belonging (Barbalet, 1999; Klapp, 1986; Winter, 2002).

Consequently, in my previous book (Finkielsztein, 2021), I did not use the term “boredom” as a close synonym of idleness, rest, apathy, or monotony. Instead, as basic coordinates of the experience, I used the sense of meaninglessness, lack of engagement/attention, lack of agency, and the failure of expectations. Many boredom researchers and scholars have claimed meaning deficit to be a basic characteristic of the feeling of boredom (Barbalet, 1999; Danckert & Eastwood, 2020; Martin et al., 2006; Svendsen, 2005; van Tilburg & Igou, 2012). As Viktor Frankl (2000) famously claimed, humans constantly “search for meaning”, and are “addicted to meaning” (Baumeister, 1991, p. 358)—boredom “can be described as meaning withdrawal, in analogy with drug withdrawal, as a feeling of discomfort that signals that our need for meaning is not being met” (Svendsen, 2016, p. 213). Boredom is thus associated with “the meaning vacuum” (Baumeister, 1991) and constitutes a certain kind of frustration, an “irritable distress after a wish collided with an unyielding reality” (Jeronimus & Laceulle, 2017)—a meaning frustration, that is, an emotional reaction to the unfulfilled need for meaning or the unsuccessful construction of meaning. Certainly, we may feel bored with activities that we perceive to be very meaningful, such as babysitting (Rogge, 2011) or lectures (Finkielsztein, 2013), or highly engaged with something that we see as ultimately meaningless (such as binge watching; Danckert & Eastwood, 2020, p. 53). This is why I connect boredom rather with personally‐perceived lack of meaning in a concrete situation rather than meaningfulness in general. As I have argued elsewhere:

There is no possibility for one to be bored and simultaneously to perceive the situation as personally meaningful. If one feels bored, it means that one does not assess the particular situation as personally valuable, and would prefer to do something else instead. If one truly finds meaning in one's occupation, one never gets bored by it. If one feels bored, it means that, at this very moment and in the specific circumstances, one does not evaluate the task/activity/situation as meaningful. The task as such may remain ‘objectively’ meaningful to somebody who performs it, but carrying it out can be, at times, boring—under certain conditions, in particular situations.

(Finkielsztein, 2021, p. 75)

Moreover, what one sees as meaningful or boring is a matter of socialization, socially‐transmitted and culturally‐dependent values and perceptions and not only personal whims. Yet, in general, for one to perceive something as meaningful, one needs a purpose, a justification (perceived value), and a sense of efficacy (agency; Baumeister, 1991). Boredom strikes when there are some problems with purpose (sense of pointlessness) and efficacy (lack of agency). As Danckert and Eastwood (2020) stated, “as its root, boredom is a crisis of agency” (p. 48; also in Eastwood & Gorelik, 2021, p. 112). If people feel no control over their actions and cannot externalize their needs and/or desires, they will have difficulties with ascribing meaning to an action. Sometimes a mere sense of control can keep boredom at bay. Boredom, therefore, is strictly associated with constraints on one's actions, which are usually socially defined. In that vein, boredom may be also connected with the failure of expectations (Conrad, 1997). The incongruence between what we need/want/expect and what is possible may result in disappointment and boredom. Later, I will demonstrate this connection with many examples. One of my theses is that the concept of expectations, so proliferated in late modernity, is vastly responsible for establishing the society of boredom.

Within relational sociology, agency is defined as “the temporally constructed engagement” (Emirbayer & Mische, 1998, p. 970), that is, one's engagement in the social world around them. In that connection, I also conceive of boredom as a lack of engagement. One is basically disconnected with one's social environment, and is disinterested in and/or bereft of social interactions. This is, by the way, the original meaning of the Latin “inter‐esse” (from which originated English “interest” and “interesting”)—to be between. The lack of interest thus means disconnection, being unable to relate to something/someone. Boredom alienates, turns us off from social engagement, and usually ends the interaction (Darden & Marks, 1999; Musharbash, 2007).

The last thing that should be mentioned here is that there are various kinds of boredom from the most volatile to the most severe. In the literature, the most basic distinction is between state/situational boredom and existential/profound boredom, which is reflected to some extent in English by the difference between boredom and ennui (Goodstein, 2005, p. 23). The former is a short‐lived and relatively superficial emotion/feeling that is a reaction to a concrete situation, task, or external circumstance that usually slips away along with the removal of its causes (Healy, 1984; Irvine, 2001; Kuhn, 1976; Toohey, 2011). The second kind is a long‐lasting mood deeply rooted in the individual's self. In that situation, boredom is not associated with a concrete situation but is a generalized attitude towards reality and involves a pessimistic view of human existence—boring and meaningless is not the situation at hand but one's whole life and/or life in general. However, there is also an intermediate kind of boredom, which I call chronic boredom. As I have explained elsewhere:

This kind of boredom affects only one sphere of the individual's life, leaving the rest of it untouched. One can be extremely bored, for instance, with one's job or studies, and still not be existentially bored, as one finds his/her leisure time or family life enjoyable and rewarding. Such boredom … is a consequence of the accumulation of situational boredoms due to its frequent repetition in similar circumstances associated with a particular sphere of one's activity.

(Finkielsztein, 2021, pp. 79–80)

My idea of chronic boredom resembles the concept of “chronified situational boredom” conceptualized by Josefa Ros Velasco (2022) and remains in sharp opposition to its psychological or psychodynamic understanding. This kind of boredom is not a matter of personal characteristics but social circumstances. It is also not a profound mood being the opposition to situational/state boredom but an intermediate type—more serious than situational and much less severe than existential one.

In this book I focus on situational and chronic boredom, for the most part leaving existential boredom to philosophers. In general, boredom will be conceptualized as an outcome of the incongruence between social reality and (a) human needs, including the need for meaning, relations with others, belonging, and sense of agency, and (b) human desires, expectations, and aspirations.

The Structure of the Book

As these are expectations that make books boring, readers need a clear picture of what they can and cannot expect from this book. It definitely does not cover all the subjects that one may imagine being associated with the phrase “sociology of boredom”, but includes a series of interconnected essays dealing with a set of social spheres (consumption, work, religion, social relations, violence) and topics (utopian thinking, social structure) in which boredom plays an active role. Chapter 1 shows links between the onset of modernity and the emergence of boredom as democratized phenomenon. Subsequent chapters constitute an exploration of boredom in various spheres of social life describing its causes, mechanics, and outcomes. The recurrent themes and claims include: (a) the social character of boredom, (b) boredom as a function of relations between an individual and their social circumstances or internalized socially prevalent perceptions of social reality, and (c) boredom as distinctively prompted by the failure of socially‐produced expectations, aspirations, appetites, and desires that due to socially‐dependent factors remain unmet or unfulfilled.

Chapter 1, “Boredom and Modernity”, presents two approaches towards the historicity of boredom: essentialism (boredom is a timeless phenomenon) and constructivism (boredom is a social construction of modernity). I propose a division between soft essentialism, hard essentialism, and constructivism, where hard essentialists contend that boredom is timeless and part of human biology and evolutionary heritage, while soft essentialists view it as an inherent quality of humans as cultural beings, an outcome of the condition of being human. Soft essentialism is a perspective found within philosophy and literary studies, which claim that boredom under various labels has always existed in human culture and experience. Chapter 1 also discusses the anthropological perspective, which advocates that the origins of boredom are associated with the shift to a sedentary and agricultural lifestyle (the Neolithic Revolution), and that boredom was a reaction to unnatural, from an evolutionary point of view, circumstances of human life. I claim here that boredom is not timeless but has a history, yet, that history is far longer than the word “boredom”, and began with abandoning the hunter‐gatherer lifestyle. I would like, however, to propose a new approach that breaks with the dichotomic division between essentialism and constructivism—historicism.

The second part of the chapter is devoted to unravelling the constructivist standpoint claiming that boredom is a uniquely modern phenomenon with no previous precedents (hard constructivism) or that boredom has been democratized in modernity, existing previously only marginally and in a trivial form. Boredom in this approach constitutes a defining feature of modernity and is a socially‐constructed phenomenon associated with a set of social changes and circumstances including disenchantment with the world (secularization and de‐ritualization), rationalization (industrialization and bureaucratization), individualization (the atrophy of community), and anomie. Boredom is a result of demagification of human experience and the replacement of the meaningful by mere function. Boredom constitutes one of the irrationalities of rationality and is a side effect of the transition between substantial rationality (pre‐modernity) and formal rationality (modernity). The cases of industrial, school, leisure, and romantic boredom are presented. The chapter concludes with a discussion of the Durkheimian notion of anomie (social state of normlessness), and I propose a division of boredom parallel to Durkheim's typology of suicides (egoistic, fatalistic, and anomic boredom). Boredom is treated as a result of anomie, which is a persistent aspect of life in modern times, as central to anomic situations is weariness of endless aspirations that reality can never satisfy, which is ultimately highly boredom‐inducing due to the failure of life to meet expectations and the state of indeterminacy of the axiological order.

Chapter 2, “Late Modern Boredom”, aims to provide a comprehensive overview of two late modern modalities of boredom: busy boredom and consumer boredom. The former describes the inner boredom felt despite the busy lifestyle of the current achievement society. The accelerated social life leaves little space for standing still, and people occupy their time with overworking and an abundance of social placebos—false remedies to boredom (one of which is using smart devices). In the chapter, I reinterpret the notion of smart boredom, giving it the meaning opposite to the popular one—smart devices do not cure our boredom, they merely push our small everyday boredoms away for a while, contributing to the emergence of a deeper mood of chronic boredom that gradually takes root in us due to accumulation of unresolved situational boredoms. The chapter also provides a concise presentation of the theory of boredom in the informational society by Orin Klapp.

The second part of the chapter, in contrast to the official discourse of boredom in consumer capitalism, poses the thesis that boredom, which is allegedly the villain and the main antagonist of consumer capitalism, is in fact its main driving force, and most powerful ally, meticulously employed for the system's benefit. Through a sequence of social and cultural shifts and freeloading on humans' natural predispositions, the system has redefined the relation between human beings and their external environment and has manufactured a new type of human, homo consumens, who is socialized to be passive, driven by desires and dreams, and addicted to novelty, constant change, a ceaseless stream of stimuli, and pleasure. Boredom enables the smooth functioning of the system. Bored consumers buy more and are led by desires that promise to alleviate the feeling. Consumer capitalism is devoted to fighting the very boredom that it actively produces, amplifies, or invites by consumer socialization towards boredom. The entire entertainment/culture industry, the boredom‐preventing industry, is in fact the boredom industry, whose institutions are preoccupied with mitigating the consumer's (natural, situational) boredom, but—at the same time—push this light type of boredom into the subliminal realm, where it has time to accumulate and takes root unnoticed, becoming a subliminal mood of consumer capitalism.

Chapter 3, “Boredom and Social Inequality”, advocates the view, contrary to the common psychologically‐driven one, that boredom is a group phenomenon, that is, is a matter of shared social position. Although representatives of all social strata are vulnerable to boredom, their understanding and perception of it may be class‐specific, that is, manifestations of boredom among different social classes are not identical in their frequency, causes/antecedents, forms/character, the way they are felt/experienced, and strategies that are employed to prevent, cope with, or remedy it may differ. The chapter, unlike the previous tradition associating boredom mostly with the upper class, discusses the boredom of various marginalized groups—the unemployed and the homeless, refugees, and women. There is a straightforward connection between occupying a marginal position in society and the experience of boredom associated with systemic constraints (inhibition of actions), lack of agency (lack of control and freedom of choice), and the sense of powerlessness. Boredom of such groups is also exacerbated by the severe failure of expectations, as a marginal position is also a matter of unfulfilled aspirations towards an inclusion in the mainstream. The marginalized experience of anomic boredom is characterized by not belonging, lack of a stable social identity, a sense of displacement, and indeterminacy. Such a situation is illustrated by the concept of waithood, a limbo state of suspension between childhood and adulthood. The liminal case described in the chapter is feminine boredom, as women were (or are) marginalized but do not always share all the traits of other marginalized groups, as their boredom is usually associated with relative affluence and caused by unfulfilled aspirations for public significance, control, and freedom, and equality (boredom here is a case of inhibition of becoming).

The second part of the chapter discusses the issue of marginalization on the global level and, employing the theory of Immanuel Wallerstein, advocates the stance that boredom can be an outcome of the troubled relationship between the centre and peripheries in the capitalistic world‐system. This kind of boredom, which I call peripheral boredom, is caused by unfulfilled aspirations of the peripheries to share the affluence of the centre. The here‐and‐now of the everyday life of the marginalized becomes totally uninteresting in comparison to what they can imagine as an alternative due to exposure to Western cultural traits (consumerism or the idea of progress; linear time) via the media and the internet. In that connection, boredom is a matter of colonial encounters. I argue that boredom is one more civilizational disease that now has been globalized. Colonialism has deprived Indigenous populations of their traditional affluence, destroying economic and social grounds for their cultural life and leaving them poor, dependent, marginal, in anomie, and with expectations previously unknown in their cultures. The chapter describes the notion of peripheral boredom using the examples of Niger, Egypt, Ethiopia, and Georgia, and Indigenous boredom among Australian Aborigines and Native Americans.

Chapter 4, “Workplace Boredom”, deals with the vocational aspect of boredom. It starts with the claim that the mere concept of work is a bit boring in itself. The busy mindset and the compulsion to work are civilizational diseases, and the current capitalistic work ethic is not natural, but something that we now call “laziness” is. The idea of work is learned and got used to through the process of a long socialization and training. Our current approach to work is related to: (a) agriculture, which accustomed people to scarcity and hard work, on which all farmers' lives have been based, and (b) modern capitalism, which has proliferated the idea of a job for monetary gain being the aim in and of itself. The crusade to make work a moral issue and to ennoble it, along with the moral condemnation of laziness, has been introduced to eradicate natural energy‐saving mechanisms that prevent people from overwork (or prevent them from being utilized for that purpose). The modern work ethic has been also introduced as a new way of exploiting many people for the benefit of a few. All philosophies trying to convince us what a marvellous thing our work is and what a privilege, honour, and opportunity it is to be able to do our work efficiently or at all are calculated to ease our exploitation. In a word, I advocate the stance that the concept and practice of work under the modern capitalist regime is boring—first of all, due to its compulsory character. Work is not natural for human beings, and spending long hours each day at work is not congruent with our evolutionary heritage. Boredom constitutes the response to such an incongruence.

Not only is the concept of work boring, but actual work is as well. The second part of the chapter is devoted to the exploration of various possible causes of workplace boredom, which is employed as a close synonym of job disengagement. The chapter analyses direct as well as systemic factors contributing to work‐related boredom. Direct factors encompass personal characteristics, task characteristics, the relationship between personal and work characteristics (person‐job fit), and the work environment. Systemic factors include bureaucratization in the form of managerialism, secularization of work (lack of calling), identity disturbance (anomic work‐related boredom), precarious employment (lack of belonging), redundant work (bullshit jobs), and commodification of feelings (emotional labour). I propose the development of a Baumeister‐inspired typology of approaches towards boredom and show that the idea of work as a calling has been superseded by the idea of work as a job, and work as a career by work as an adventure (the last category is my proposal for supplementing the typology). I also claim that this shift is responsible for much work‐related boredom. The feeling can also be associated with vocational identity and its disturbance or crisis. Boredom is more likely to emerge in those activities that are perceived as further from an individual's core identity, for which more identity work is needed to make them meaningful. Boredom might constitute an emotional response to identity disturbance, which occurs when the roles that are offered are not perceived as central to one's identity and do not meet one's expectations of their imagined role/calling (the struggle between the ideal self and the imposed self). Work‐related boredom may also be associated with precarious employment. I claim that the unique mixture of lack of prospects/future and lack of belonging characteristic of precarious employment may ultimately eventuate in job disengagement. Boredom may also be characteristic of so‐called “bullshit jobs” (Graeber, 2018)—those jobs which are pointless and performed simply for the sake of keeping one working. Boredom is also analysed in the context of emotional labour (Hochschild, 1983), suppressing and/or advancing particular on‐work emotions in order to accommodate to feeling rules. Emotional labour (including suppressing on‐the‐job boredom) leads to emotional dissonance and results in a lack of commitment. The last section of the chapter briefly expounds on the consequences of boredom, such as empty labour (performing non‐work‐related activities at work), boreout and burnout, and counter‐productive work behaviours (including various workplace boredom‐coping strategies).

Chapter 5, “Religious Boredom”, is aimed at analysing the connections between boredom and religion. The chapter presents boredom as a strictly moral issue, as the usual consequence of boredom is depicted as sin, and it is positively correlated with numerous violations of moral and legal codes. Boredom has also been presented as the foundation of all religions and religion as an ultimate remedy for it, as it is universally perceived as a meaning‐producing social institution. In that view, boredom constitutes a kind of a‐religious emotion that contradicts all religious emotional regimes. In the chapter, I claim that the first step towards the emergence of religious boredom was the replacement of magic by religion, and the institutionalization of religion. Another was secularization (especially, the secularization of consciousness; Berger, 2011). Religious boredom originates from the contact of religion with the secular civilization and from the domination of the secular over the sacred in social life. Boredom, caused by the failure of expectations and lack of a sense of meaning, has been responsible for heresies, for establishing new religions and denominations, in a word, for religious change. The second part of the chapter is devoted to two major modalities of religious boredom, the boredom associated with the sphere of religion and spirituality: acedia and church boredom. Acedia is a kind of profound religious boredom conceptualized in Christianity in late antiquity as a lack of care, indifference, and disconnection with the sacred. The section analyses the inter‐connections between acedia and boredom, showing that both concepts share much of their phenomenology. Ancient descriptions of monks' acedia demonstrate that acedic individuals constantly wait for stimulation, change, and events, are not engaged in what they are doing, cannot focus attention on any particular task, frequently change the activity, and are listless and drowsy but restless at the same time. The second modality, church boredom, constitutes a kind of situational or chronic religious boredom associated primarily with various aspects of religious rituals and practices epitomized by attending a church (a place of congregation).

Chapter 6, “Boredom and Social Change”, interprets boredom as an incentive for social change. Boredom is credited with precipitating the rise and decline of civilizations, heresies, reformation movements, the rise of nationalisms and radical political movements, and all kinds of revolutions and wars. Boredom can lead to social mobilization, playing a significant role in both the rise and fall of social, political, or military movements. Usually, the promise of breaking out of the “terminal boredom” of mundane social life through a sense of community and purpose, and the prospects of activism and change—any change sometimes—serves as a powerful recruitment tool. Boredom with politics can be partially responsible for the growing popularity of and social support for extreme political parties and figures and be a threat to democracy. Boredom is also believed to be a significant motivational factor in all kinds of violent and destructive behaviours, such as massacres, pogroms, terrorism, torture, wars, and riots and revolutions. In the chapter, I take a close look at wars, terrorism, and revolutions. Boredom is a factor that leads to war, as war promises to effectively relieve boredom. Boredom has also been found to be the “most powerful recruiting tool” for terrorist organizations. In the chapter, I also present the thesis that boredom is a vital contributory factor to the revolutionary situation and in this way to actual revolution. In a weak sense, accumulated boredom is a background mood that enables the occurrence of a revolutionary situation; in a strong sense, accumulated boredom is the direct emotional cause of the outbreak. I also propose the explanatory scheme of how boredom might be involved in the aetiology of revolutions based on Gurr's theory of relative deprivation (the discrepancy between expectations and reality). I would like to hypothesize that boredom in this scheme is an intermittent phase of revolutionary mobilization: expectations make the reality unsatisfactory, disappointing, and tedious, and boredom is an ultimate outcome of such a failure of expectations.

Chapter 7, “Boredom and Utopia”, analyses various connections between boredom and utopian projects (both eutopian and dystopian as well as literary and real‐life ones). Utopia is defined, following Darko Suvin, as a project that designs realities radically different from the authors'/proponents’ one. Boredom is indicated to be a primary source of the utopian impulse and one of the feelings that haunt inhabitants of utopias. The chapter scrutinizes various reasons for which utopias of various kinds can be boring, such as lack of social change, social conservatism, affluence, or overcontrol and lack of agency. There is nothing to strive for in a utopia, and all emotions and dissent are inhibited and repressed. Boredom is prohibited in utopia because it constitutes a sign of ingratitude, dissatisfaction, and possibly resentment. Passive contentment is the most recommended state for utopian adherents. Boredom may also have an alienating effect and be detrimental to community—and therefore is usually the end of the utopia. The next section of the chapter is devoted to the most frequently prescribed techniques for preventing boredom by utopian proponents (work, leisure, community). The last part of the chapter is focused on boredom of the real‐time, “achieved” utopias—the kibbutz and state communism.

Chapter 8